Back to Journals » Journal of Pain Research » Volume 7

Pain-related mood influences pain perception differently in fibromyalgia and multiple sclerosis

Authors Borg C, Padovan C, Thomas-Antérion C, Chanial C, Sanchez A, Godot M, Peyron R, De Parisot O, Laurent B

Received 31 May 2013

Accepted for publication 16 July 2013

Published 22 January 2014 Volume 2014:7 Pages 81—87

DOI https://doi.org/10.2147/JPR.S49236

Checked for plagiarism Yes

Review by Single anonymous peer review

Peer reviewer comments 3



Céline Borg,1–3 Catherine Padovan,4,5 Catherine Thomas-Antérion,1,2 Céline Chanial,1 Anaïs Sanchez,1,5 Marion Godot,6 Roland Peyron,1,6,7 Odile De Parisot,5 Bernard Laurent1,6,7

1Neurology/Neuropsychology, Center Memory of Resources and Research Unit, North Saint-Etienne University Hospital Center, Avenue Albert Raimond, Saint-Priest-en-Jarez, France; 2Study of Cognitive Mechansims Laboratory, 3Psychology Unit, University Lumière of Lyon 2, Bron, France; 4Brain Dynamics and Cognition, Lyon Neuroscience Research Center and Vinatier Hospital Center, Bron, France; 5Germaine Revel Center, Dargoire, France; 6Pain Center, North Saint-Etienne University Hospital Center, Saint-Priest-en-Jarez, France; 7Central Integration of Pain, Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, Bron, France

Abstract: In patients, the perception of pain intensity may be influenced by the subjective representation of their disease. Although both multiple sclerosis (MS) and fibromyalgia (FM) possibly include chronic pain, they seem to elicit different disease representations because of the difference in their respective etiology, the former presenting evidence of underlying lesions as opposed to the latter. Thus, we investigated whether patients with FM differed from patients with MS with respect to their perception of “own” pain as well as others' pain. In addition, the psychological concomitant factors associated with chronic pain were considered. Chronic pain patients with FM (n=13) or with MS (n=13) participated in this study. To assess specific pain-related features, they were contrasted with 12 other patients with MS but without chronic pain and 31 controls. A questionnaire describing imaginary painful situations showed that FM patients rated situations applied to themselves as less painful than did the controls. Additionally, pain intensity attributed to facial expressions was estimated as more intense in FM compared with the other groups of participants. There is good evidence that the mood and catastrophizing reactions expressed in FM differentially modulated the perception of pain according to whether it was their own pain or other's pain.

Keywords: chronic pain, self and other's perspective, imaginary pain, facial expression

Introduction

The perception of pain includes both an objective experience made of somatic sensory processes and a subjective experience consisting of affective–motivational features, as the consequence of actual or potential tissue damage (nociception). Thus, individuals base the estimation of their pain on objective sensory criteria, but also, they learn how to graduate their sensations according to personal values and beliefs. For instance, a study found that people who assessed their past and future health as poor reported more present pain.1 Generally, the awareness of inner self is a complex and multifactorial variable, and self-assessment of one’s own pain is a challenging task. People tend to make biased evaluations of themselves and to use different criteria for judging themselves and others. For instance, normal volunteers reported a much more positive opinion of themselves than of others.2,3 In the pain domain, studies on empathy have shown that healthy participants scored pain with higher intensities when they experienced it than when they observed another individual in pain.4 These results are consistent with the view that empathy is not a simple resonance of affect between the self and others5 but requires a more complex perspective. These results also suggest a large influence of subjective mechanisms in the attribution of pain intensity in normal volunteers. Accordingly, pain in self and in others have shown different patterns of activation of the sensory discriminative areas of the brain6,7 but shares similar neural activations in the anterior cingulate cortex and anterior insula, both are known to participate in the (subjective) affective–motivational component of pain.8,9

Chronic diseases without evidence of lesions in the discriminative system but with psychogenic dysfunctions may be considered as interesting models to investigate how chronic pain influences the evaluation of “own” pain and others’ pain. Fibromyalgia (FM) may be considered as the prototype for these diseases, contributing to pain persistence10,11 and showing abnormal activation in the nociceptive system, particularly, in the anterior cingulate cortex.12,13 By contrast, multiple sclerosis (MS) is a disease that is systematically associated with lesions of the central nervous system and that is frequently associated with pain symptoms. Intuitively, it seems evident that being in chronic pain or having a chronic disease may induce more empathy towards others’ pain. Additionally, it has been shown that chronic pain is accompanied by psychological symptoms, such as a propensity for catastrophizing, as well as by levels of anxiety and depression that are higher than normal.1416 Therefore, this paper examined the impact of chronic pain on the perception of pain by paying particular attention to the psychological factors concomitant to the chronic pain.

The present study specifically addressed whether or not the presence of chronic pain has an effect on how people estimate painful experiences in themselves and in others. The study was initiated in patients with FM and was extended to a matched population of patients with MS and chronic pain. To discriminate the respective effects of chronic pain and the disease itself, two groups of MS patients were formed according to whether they suffered from pain (the MS-P group) or not (the MS-NP group). The FM group and both groups of MS patients were matched to a group of healthy volunteers. Since FM is often accompanied by behavioral and psychological symptoms, such as a propensity for catastrophizing,17 as well as by levels of anxiety and depression that are higher than normal,18,11 we assumed that the levels of catastrophizing, anxiety, and depression would be higher in this group than in the MS-P group. In addition, anxiety, depression, and the “dramatization of pain” have been shown to increase the pain experience;19 consequently, we speculated that patients with FM would have a greater tendency to overestimate pain than would the other groups. In other words, patients with FM would have more difficulty discriminating the different intensities of pain in various imaginary situations and displayed from facial expressions. Precisely, this difficulty should express itself in a higher score when they would have to imagine the intensity of pain felt in various imaginary situations, especially concerning their own pain; we further speculated that the FM group should assess painful facial expressions as more intense compared with the other group.

Methods

Participants

Thirty-eight patients and 31 healthy participants gave written informed consent, according to the Declaration of Helsinki. The sociodemographic data of the participants are presented in Table 1.

Table 1 Participants’ characteristics
Notes: Data represent mean (standard deviation) values. FM = the group of patients with fibromyalgia; MS-P = the group of multiple sclerosis patients with pain; MS-NP = the group of multiple sclerosis patients without pain. *Significantly differed from controls (P<0.05). Education levels: 1= primary education; 2= secondary education; 3= college education.
Abbreviations: MMSE, Mini Mental State Examination; MOCA, Montreal Cognitive Assessment.

Thirteen patients with FM were referred by the Pain Unit of the Saint-Etienne Hospital, Saint-Priest-en-Jarez, France and met the diagnostic criteria for FM.20 The mean duration of the disease was 5.07 years (standard deviation [SD]: ±3.01 years).

Twenty-five patients with MS were selected from the Department of Neurology at the Saint-Etienne University Hospital and the Germaine Revel Center at Saint-Maurice-sur-Dargoire, France. All of these patients fulfilled the inclusion criteria for MS as defined by the United States National Multiple Sclerosis Society in 1996.21 Five patients had primary progressive MS, nine patients had relapsing remitting MS, and nine patients secondary progressive MS. They were split into the two groups MS-P and MS-NP according to whether they had pain or not in order to discriminate the respective effects of chronic pain and of the disease itself.

Pain falls into two categories: primary pain and secondary pain. For the present study, we included only MS patients with primary pain, in other words, with neuropathic pain, according to the criteria of O’Connor et al.22 Patients, especially those in the MS group, with secondary pain, such as pain of psychological origin or pain associated with spasticity symptoms, were not included.

For both FM and MS patients, the exclusion criteria were as follows: other neurologic diseases, history of psychiatric illness, history of head trauma, history of alcohol or drug abuse, or present use of narcotics. Isolated mood disorder was not an exclusion criterion for these patients. All participants in the FM and MS groups had unchanged doses of medication.

Since cognitive impairments have been reported in both FM 23,24 and MS,25 patients were tested with the Mini Mental State Examination Test (MMSE)26,27 and the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MOCA)28 (Table 1).

The exclusion criteria for healthy participants included any current psychiatric or neurological disorder. None of the control participants were taking psychoactive medication. The patient and control groups showed no significant differences with respect to age and educational level (Table 1).

Materials and procedure

Mood disorders and pain catastrophizing assessment

Depressive mood and anxiety were evaluated using the Self-assessment Questionnaire of Depression (QD2A)29 for depressive symptomatology and the Anxiety Scale Questionnaire,30 respectively. The cutoff thresholds were ≥7 for the QD2A and ≥5 for the Anxiety Scale Questionnaire. The French Canadian version of the Pain Catastrophizing Scale (PCS-CF)31 was used to assess pain catastrophizing.

Situational Pain Questionnaire

The procedure and tasks used in the present study, to assess the perception of pain in FM and MS patients compared to controls, were similar to those used in the Danziger et al study.32 Firstly, to evaluate how FM and MS patients estimate their own pain sensitivity and the pain sensitivity of others, we used the French version of the Situational Pain Questionnaire (SPQ)-30 items version.33 This includes 15 descriptions of painful events (eg, “The dentist drilled one of your teeth without anesthesia”) and 15 nonpainful events (eg, “Someone is bitten by a mosquito”). Participants completed two versions of the SPQ on the same sheet – one version interrogating their imagining of their own pain sensations in different situations (SPQself) and the second, the pain sensations they imagined in a normal other individual of the same gender and age (SPQother).

The items were evaluated using a numerical rating scale ranging from 1 (not noticeable) to 10 (worst possible pain) and yielded a discrimination score and a response bias score. The discrimination score P(A) indicates the ability of subjects to differentiate painful and painless situations. The P(A) can vary between 0 and 1: a score of 0 means no discrimination, a score of 0.5 is equivalent to a choice by chance, and a score of 1.0 represents perfect discrimination. The response bias (B) score indicates the extent to which the situations can be considered as painful. The less painful the situations are considered, the higher the B score. Both the P(A) and the B score were calculated as two scores, respectively P(A)self and P(A)other, and Bself and Bother.

Faces expressing pain

Additionally, the ability of patients to estimate the intensity of the pain of others from their facial expressions was assessed using the Sensitivity to Expressions of Pain Test (STEP Test).34 This test consists of video clips showing facial expressions of patients undergoing different active and passive movements of their shoulders, some being painful. These clips were sampled and classified according to the intensity of pain reported using the Facial Action Coding System.35 Facial Action Coding System is a method of describing facial movements developed by psychologists Ekman and Friesen in 1978.35

Sixty 1-second sequences were randomly presented, with 20 depicting no pain, 20 depicting strong pain, and 20 depicting moderate pain. Three pretest sequences were used as examples before the 60 items were presented. The participants were asked to determine whether the sequence represented “no pain” (score 0), “moderate pain” (score 1), or “strong pain” (score 2). The scores were based on the nonparametric model of the signal detection theory, which provided two scores for discrimination P(A) and two scores for response bias (B): the difference between “no pain” and “moderate pain” expressions (P[A]NM and BNM); and the difference between “no pain” and “strong pain,” expressions (P[A]NS and BNS). P(A) and B values were calculated in similar manner as those for the SPQ. Note here that the response bias scores of the STEP Test were computed in such a manner that the higher the response bias, the higher the pain inferred. In contrast, the analyses conducted for the SPQ let to interpret the results as follows: the less painful the situations are considered, the higher the B score.

Data analyses

For each group, the mean scores were collected from the QD2A scale, the Anxiety Scale Questionnaire, and the Pain Catastrophinzing scale. Additionally, as mentioned, the P(A) and the B scores were calculated on data collected from the SPQ and STEP tests. Considering the small number of patients, the P(A) and the B scores were each rank-transformed36 and treated to a separate analysis of variance (ANOVA) and of covariance (ANCOVA) with the group as the between-groups factor; the depression, anxiety, and catastrophizing variables were used as covariates. Post hoc comparisons were carried out using the Bonferroni adjustment.

Results

Mood and catastrophizing assessments

As described in Table 2, all patients with chronic pain (ie, the FM and MS-P groups) showed higher depression and anxiety scores than did the controls. The patients with FM had a higher catastrophizing score than did the controls, whereas the patients with MS-P did not differ from controls. As the likelihood of anxiety, depression, and catastrophizing was increased among the FM patients, these variables were used as covariates in the later analyses.

Table 2 Pain and mood assessments
Notes: Data represents mean (standard deviation) values. FM = the group of patients with fibromyalgia; MS-P = the group of multiple sclerosis patients with pain; MS-NP = the group of multiple sclerosis patients without pain. *Significantly differed from controls (P<0.05); **patients with pain significantly differed from controls and MS patients without pain.
Abbreviation: PCS-CF, French Canadian Pain Catastrophizing Scale.

Rating of imaginary situations (SPQ)

Firstly, the ANOVAs revealed a main effect of group (F[3,64] =6.41) (P=0.0007) for the version of the SPQ applied to the “self:” the FM patients showed a significantly lower P(A) than did the controls, P(A)self =0.76 (standard deviation [SD]: 0.09) versus 0.88 (SD: 0.07), respectively (P<0.0001). This pattern of results indicates that the FM patients were less able than others to differentiate between painful and nonpainful situations that applied to themselves (see Figure 1). Though ANOVA analyses revealed significant differences, no effect was revealed with the ANCOVA analyses. This means that the significant differences observed between the groups were due to the psychological factors assessed.

Figure 1 SPQ discrimination score for “self” pain.
Notes: Variability appears in terms of standard errors. ***Significant differences (P<0.0001) were reported.
Abbreviations: FM, fibromyalgia patients; MS-P, multiple sclerosis patients with pain; MS-NP, multiple sclerosis patients with no pain; SPQ, Situational Pain Questionnaire; TM, normal controls.

Similarly, ANOVAs revealed a main effect of group, F(3,64) =3.02 (P=0.03), when the B score was considered as the dependent variable. The FM patients showed significantly higher B scores than did the controls (Bself =6.07 [SD: 1.73] versus 5.04 [SD: 1.31], respectively) (P=0.03) or than the MS-P patients (Bself =5.13 [SD: 1.52]) (P=0.0002), indicating that they were less able than others to consider imaginary stimuli that applied to themselves as painful (see Figure 2). Once again, the ANCOVA analyses revealed no effect.

Figure 2 SPQ response bias for “self” pain.
Notes: Variability appears in terms of standard errors. *Significant differences (P<0.05) were reported.
Abbreviations: FM, fibromyalgia patients; MS-P, multiple sclerosis patients with pain; MS-NP, multiple sclerosis patients with no pain; SPQ, Situational Pain Questionnaire; TM, normal controls.

Finally, no significant difference was observed in the ratings of the SPQ applied to others.

Facial expressions of pain (STEP test)

Differences were mainly observed in the P[A]NM score (between faces expressing “moderate pain” and those expressing “no pain”) (F[3,64] =3.09) (P=0.03), and significant differences were found between the FM patients and the controls (P=0.01), between the MS-P patients and the controls (P=0.03), and with a trend toward significance between the MS-NP patients and the controls (P=0.06). This effect reflects that all patients, even MS-NP patients, were less able than controls to correctly distinguish subtle differences of intensity in facial expression (see Table 3).

The B score between “no pain” and “strong pain” on one hand and between “no pain” and “moderate pain” on the other hand showed a main effect of group (F[3,64] =3.54 [P=0.01] and F[3,64] =2.8 [P=0.04], respectively). Only the FM patients differed from the controls (“no pain” vs “strong pain”) (P=0.0009) (Table 3) and from the other groups (“no pain” vs “moderate pain”) (P<0.01), in their manner of inferring increased pain intensities from facial expression.

Table 3 Sensitivity to facial expressions of pain (STEP test)
Notes: The discrimination score, P(A), indicates the extent to which participants were able to differentiate facial expressions. The response bias (B) score indicates patients’ tendency to infer pain from the facial expressions. Two discriminations scores and two bias scores were analyzed: NS (between “no pain” and “strong pain” expressions) and NM (between “no pain” and “moderate pain” expressions). The data represent mean (errors standard) values. *Significant differences between patients and controls (P<0.05).
Abbreviations: FM, fibromyalgia patients; MS-P, multiple sclerosis patients with pain; MS-NP, multiple sclerosis patients with no pain.

When depression, anxiety, and catastrophizing were used as covariates, no significant difference was observed between the groups.

Discussion

FM patients showed significantly more mental distress, including depression and anxiety, than did the healthy controls. These findings replicate other studies showing a link between FM, anxiety and depression.11,37 Interestingly, these abnormalities were shared with the group of patients having MS and chronic pain. In contrast, the patients with FM were the only group of patients to overdramatize their pain in comparison with the control group. Therefore, within the field of this study, catastrophizing seems specific to FM but not to chronic pain. It has been reported that pain catastrophizing is significantly correlated with increased activity in the brain areas subserving anticipation, attention, and the emotional aspects of pain,13 suggesting possible cognitive or fearful biases towards potentially painful events. Increased catastrophizing as well as the presence of anxiety and depression in FM should have led patients to assign a higher negative value to external painful stimuli.38 Contrary to these predictions, the FM patients imagined pain situations applied to themselves as less painful than did the other groups. This downplay in the representation of pain intensity for external events applied to themselves (as described in the SPQ, for example, pain felt at the dentist) may suggest that they considered these painful situations to be less intense than their everyday pain. Conversely, when imaging pain in others, the performance of patients with FM did not differ from the control group, suggesting an intact ability for empathy. Similar abilities to properly assess pain in others have been previously reported in the extreme clinical situation of patients who never experience pain (congenital insensitivity to pain).32 These results suggest that it is possible to adequately describe pain intensity in others through general knowledge and semantic cues, even in the absence of previous experience,32 and a similar explanatory mechanism may apply to patients with FM. Such a semantic process does not apply to their own pain in patients with MS, suggesting a primary reference to their own emotional or sensorimotor maps. This discrepancy seen between the normal judgment of others’ pain based on semantic criteria and the downregulation of their own pain intensity processes seems relatively specific to FM patients, since it was not observed in MS-P patients.

Additionally, the patients with FM did not appear to benefit from emotional cues during the presentation of facial expressions since they overrated pain intensity as compared with controls. This result differs from findings in patients with congenital insensitivity to pain who had relatively normal performances – in spite of the absence of painful experiences, they were able to detect pain appropriately out of the emotional cues induced by facial expressions.32 Since facial expressions are thought to involve the affective component of pain experience in the anterior cingulate cortex and the insula,8,24 a first hypothesis could be that patients with FM have increased sensitivity in this affective field, consistent with other experimental data.39 An alternative hypothesis could be that patients with FM show excessive empathy when evaluating facial expressions of pain because they tend to project their own pain onto the stimuli. Accordingly, patients with MS, with or without pain, were less able than controls to distinguish the different intensities of pain on faces but showed very similar B scores. As compared with FM, they had very slight and restricted abnormalities in the area of empathy. This difference between patients with MS and those with FM may be explained first by the etiology of the disease that is clearly somatic for the former and with a potent psychosomatic component for the latter.

In conclusion, our results showed that the psychological factors concomitant to pain, especially in FM, are variables that minimized the judgment of pain intensity attributed to a situation of “own” pain, as evoked by semantic cues in the FM patients. This is a quite specific pattern of response, absent in patients with chronic pain from other origins. This is a pattern of response that does not apply to judgment of pain in others, suggesting, in FM patients, a normal empathic reaction to these semantic cues. In addition, the FM patients showed an increased empathic reaction based on emotional cues, as assessed with facial expressions, suggesting a generalized enhanced sensitivity to painful events. Again, this may be a relatively specific pattern of dysfunction since it is not observed in other patients with chronic pain.

The small number of participants may constitute one of the weak points of this study; however, even with weak statistical power, the results show significant differences, highlighting the importance of considering these results. Further studies seem necessary because of the small size of the group and to more precisely test the relative contributions of discriminative, emotional, and empathic reactions in patients with FM, to specify which of these components are impaired in FM.

Acknowledgments

We thank Professor Nicolas Danziger (Paris) and Dr Kenneth Prkachin (Canada) who allowed us to use their experimental materials, the SPQ and the STEP. We additionally thank Dr Kenneth Prkachin for his valuable help in analyzing some of the results. This work received a 2011 research prize from the SFETD (French Society of Study and Treatment of Pain)/APICIL Foundation (Association of Interprofessional Foresight of Executives and Engineers of the Lyon region). This award was given beforehand to fund the study. Finally, we would like thank Fannie Carrier-Emond for the corrections in English.

Disclosure

The authors report no conflicts of interest in this work.


References

1.

Idler EL. Perceptions of pain and perceptions of health. Motiv Emot. 1993;17(3):205–224.

2.

Baumeister RF. The self. In: Gilbert DT, Fiske ST, Lindzey G, editors. Handbook of Social Psychology. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill; 1998:680–740.

3.

Pronin E, Lin DY, Ross L. The bias blind spot: Perceptions of bias in self versus others. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2002;28(3):369–381.

4.

Jackson PL, Brunet E, Meltzoff AN, Decety J. Empathy examined through the neural mechanisms involved in imagining how I feel versus how you feel pain. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44(5):752–761.

5.

Lamm C, Batson CD, Decety J. The neural substrate of human empathy: effects of perspective-taking and cognitive appraisal. J Cogn Neurosci. 2007;19(1):42–58.

6.

Ochsner KN, Zaki J, Hanelin J, et al. Your pain or mine? Common and distinct neural systems supporting the perception of pain in self and other. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2008;3(2):144–160.

7.

Singer T, Seymour B, O’Doherty J, Kaube H, Dolan RJ, Frith CD. Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain. Science. 2004;303(5661):1157–1162.

8.

Botvinick M, Jha AP, Bylsma LM, Fabian SA, Solomon PE, Prkachin KM. Viewing facial expressions of pain engages cortical areas involved in the direct experience of pain. Neuroimage. 2005;25(1):312–319.

9.

Decety J, Jackson PL. The functional architecture of human empathy. Behav Cogn Neurosci Rev. 2004;3(2):71–100.

10.

Cathébras P. Troubles Fonctionnels et Somatisation. Comment Aborder les Symptômes Médicalement Inexpliqués [Functional Disorders and Somatization. How to Deal with Medically Unexplained Symptoms]. Masson: Paris, 2006. French.

11.

Laroche F. Actualités sur la fibromyalgia [News on fibromyalgia]. Rev Rhum Ed Fr. 2009;76:529–536. French.

12.

Derbyshire SW, Jones AK, Devani P, et al. Cerebral responses to pain in patients with atypical facial pain measured by positron emission tomography. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1994;57(10):1166–1172.

13.

Gracely RH, Petzke F, Wolf JM, Clauw DJ. Functional magnetic resonance imaging evidence of augmented pain processing in fibromyalgia. Arthritis Rheum. 2002;46(5):1333–1343.

14.

Eccleston C, Crombez G, Aldrich S, Stannard C. Attention and somatic awareness in chronic pain. Pain. 1997;72(1–2):209–215.

15.

Grisart JM, Van der Linden M. Conscious and automatic uses of memory in chronic pain patients. Pain. 2001;94(3):305–313.

16.

Grisart J, Van der Linden M, Masquelier E. Controlled processes and automaticity in memory functioning in fibromyalgia patients: relation with emotional distress and hypervigilance. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2002;24(8):994–1009.

17.

Gracely RH, Geisser ME, Giesecke T, et al. Pain catastrophizing and neural responses to pain among persons with fibromyalgia. Brain. 2004;127(Pt 4):835–843.

18.

Gormsen L, Rosenberg R, Bach FW, Jensen TS. Depression, anxiety, health-related quality of life and pain in patients with chronic fibromyalgia and neuropathic pain. Eur J Pain. 2010;14(2):127.e1–127.e8.

19.

Fontaine R, Vanhaudenhuyse A, Demertzi A, Laureys S, Faymonville ME. Apport de la neuro-imagerie fonctionnelle à l’étude de la douleur [Contribution of functional neuroimaging to the study of pain]. Rev Rhum Ed Fr.

20.

Wolfe F, Clauw DJ, Fitzcharles MA, et al. The American College of Rheumatology preliminary diagnostic criteria for fibromyalgia and measurement of symptom severity. Arthritis Care Res (Hoboken). 2010;62(5):600–610.

21.

Polman CH, Reingold SC, Banwell B, et al. Diagnostic criteria for multiple sclerosis: 2010 revisions to the McDonald criteria. Ann Neurol. 2011;69(2):292–302.

22.

O’Connor AB, Schwid SR, Herrmann DN, Markman JD, Dworkin RH. Pain associated with multiple sclerosis: systematic review and proposed classification. Pain. 2008;137(1):96–111.

23.

Park DC, Glass JM, Minear M, Crofford LJ. Cognition function in fibromyalgia patients. Arthritis Rheum. 2001;44(9):2125–2133.

24.

Verdejo-García A, López-Torrecillas F, Calandre EP, Delgado-Rodríguez A, Bechara A. Executive function and decision-making in women with fibromyalgia. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2009;24(1):113–122.

25.

Chiaravalloti ND, DeLuca J. Cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis. Lancet Neurol. 2008;7(12):1139–1151.

26.

Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12(3):189–198.

27.

Kalafat M, Hugonot-Diener L, Poitrenaud J. Standardisation et étalonnage Français du “Mini Mental State” (MMS), version Greco [Standardization and calibration of the French “Mini Mental State” (MMS) GRECO version]. Rev Neuropsychol. 2003;13(2):209–236. French.

28.

Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bédirian V, et al. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2005;53(4):695–699.

29.

Pichot P, Boyet P, Pull CB, Rein W, Simon M, Thibault A. [A self evaluation inventory for depressive complaints, the QD2. II: Abridged version, QD2A]. Revue de Psychologie Appliquée. 1984;34(4):323–340. French.

30.

Goldberg D, Bridges K, Duncan-Jones P, Grayson D. Detecting anxiety and depression in general medical settings. BMJ. 1988;297(6653):897–899.

31.

French DJ, Noël M, Vigneau F, French JA, Cyr CP, Evans RT. PCS-CF: A French-language, French-Canadian adaptation of the Pain Catastrophizing Scale. Can J Behav Sci. 2005;37(3):181–192.

32.

Danziger N, Prkachin KM, Willer JC. Is pain the price of empathy? The perception of others’ pain in patients with congenital insensitivity to pain. Brain. 2006;129(Pt 9):2494–2507.

33.

Clark WC, Yang JC. Applications of sensory detection theory to problems in laboratory and clinical pain. In: Melzack R, editor. Pain Measurement and Assessment. New York, NY: Raven Press; 1983:15–25.

34.

Prkachin KM, Mass H, Mercer SR. Effects of exposure on perception of pain expression. Pain. 2004;111:8–12.

35.

Ekman P, Friesen WV. Facial Action Coding System. A Technique for the Measurement of Facial Movement. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press; 1978.

36.

Conover W, Iman RL. Rank transformation as a bridge between parametric and non parametric statistics. American Statistician. 1981;35: 124–129.

37.

Thieme K, Turk DC, Flor H. Comorbid depression and anxiety in fibromyalgia syndrome: relationship to somatic and psychosocial variables. Psychosom Med. 2004;66(6):837–844.

38.

Peters ML, Vlaeyen JW, van Drunen C. Do fibromyalgia patients display hypervigilance for innocuous somatosensory stimuli? Application of a body scanning reaction time paradigm. Pain. 2000;86(3):283–292.

39.

Piccinni A, Bazzichi L, Marazziti D, et al. Subthreshold mood symptoms in patients with fibromyalgia and rheumatoid arthritis. Clin Exp Rheumatol. 2011;29(6 Suppl 69):S55–S59.

Creative Commons License © 2014 The Author(s). This work is published and licensed by Dove Medical Press Limited. The full terms of this license are available at https://www.dovepress.com/terms.php and incorporate the Creative Commons Attribution - Non Commercial (unported, v3.0) License. By accessing the work you hereby accept the Terms. Non-commercial uses of the work are permitted without any further permission from Dove Medical Press Limited, provided the work is properly attributed. For permission for commercial use of this work, please see paragraphs 4.2 and 5 of our Terms.