Back to Journals » Psychology Research and Behavior Management » Volume 13

Food Selection Under Stress Among Undergraduate Students in Riyadh, Saudi Arabia

Authors Mohamed BA, Mahfouz MS , Badr MF 

Received 1 November 2019

Accepted for publication 14 February 2020

Published 28 February 2020 Volume 2020:13 Pages 211—221

DOI https://doi.org/10.2147/PRBM.S236953

Checked for plagiarism Yes

Review by Single anonymous peer review

Peer reviewer comments 3

Editor who approved publication: Professor Mei-Chun Cheung



Badreldin Abdelrhman Mohamed,1 Mohamed Salih Mahfouz,2 Mohamed Farouk Badr1

1Department of Community Health Sciences, College of Applied Medical Sciences, King Saud University, Riyadh 11433, Saudi Arabia; 2Department of Family and Community Medicine, Faculty of Medicine, Jazan University, Jazan 45142, Saudi Arabia

Correspondence: Mohamed Salih Mahfouz
Department of Family and Community Medicine, Faculty of Medicine, PO Box 2531, Jazan, Saudi Arabia
Email [email protected]

Background: University students might experience numerous sources of stress in their daily lives. Previous research has found that stress affects food selection patterns, but the effect might vary by country and sex. No previous study conducted in Saudi Arabia has assessed the association between stress and dietary habits among university students. Thus, this study aimed to examine the association between perceived stress and food selection patterns among undergraduate students by sex at King Saud University (KSU).
Methods: A cross-sectional study of 400 students attending the College of Applied Medical Sciences in Riyadh, Saudi Arabia, was conducted. The self-administered questionnaire that was used consisted of three sections: sociodemographic, food frequency questionnaire, and Cohen’s Perceived Stress Scale.
Results: Approximately (59.0%) of the participants reported suffering from some level of stress, with more females (64.5%) experiencing stress than males (54.0%). The univariate analysis showed that under stress, more females (68.0%) reported eating more than usual than males (49.0%), while the percentage of respondents that reported eating less than usual was higher for females (23.0%) than that for males (31.0%). The McNemar test indicated that under stress, there was an increased preference for salty flavors, which was not associated with sex. Significantly more females (82.0%) than males (64.5%) reported preferring sweets under stress. More males reported preferring bitter and savory flavors than females. Under stress, females preferred homemade food, while males preferred takeout foods. More females (68.0%) reported losing control and overeating than males (49.0%). A logistic regression showed that for female students, perceived stress was associated with frequent consumption of sweets, cake/cookies, snacks and beverages and less frequent consumption of fruits and vegetables. For males, the consumption of fast food and meat was significantly positively associated with perceived stress.
Conclusion: Perceived stress led to unhealthy changes in eating patterns in both sexes, as evidenced by reports of an increased preference for sweets, snacks and cake/cookies among females and an increased preference for fast food and meat among males. These results may be potentially important targets for interventions for stress-related food consumption.

Keywords: stress, food intake, undergraduate students, sex, Riyadh

Introduction

University entrance can be a stressful experience in view of the major life changes involved in the transition to college. University students are challenged by many stressors in their day to day lives. New social relationships, peer competition, the need to achieve high grades, and lack of time management are some of the factors that can cause or aggravate students stress. Stress causes biological and psychological changes that can alter food preferences and consumption.1,2 Individual changes in eating behavior in response to stress are not universal. Studies have shown that food selection patterns among stressed individuals can vary by country of residence,37 culture,8 sex,4,9,10 or a combination of these factors.

The eating behaviors of males and females vary in response to stress. In several studies, females have shown vulnerability to increased food consumption under stress compared to males, who tend to eat less.1,1114 In contrast, other studies have found no change in eating behavior.15 Perhaps this is because it has also been shown that specific food preferences vary by sex. For example, Oliver and Wardle16 reported that stressed females were more likely to eat snack foods such as sweets and less likely to eat fruits and vegetables. In a survey study on comfort food preferences, males reported preferring warm, hearty, meal-related foods (steak, casseroles and soup) under stress, while females preferred snack foods (ice cream and chocolate).17 Moreover, in an experimental study, women were more likely to eat sweet foods than men during a stressful video about industrial accidents. In fact, compared to controls, women ate 202% more sweet foods when stressed, and men ate 61.0% more sweet foods when stressed.11 These findings might be confounded by the fact that more women report eating fruits and vegetables, avoiding high-fat foods, and dieting than men during times of nonstress.18,19

The nutritional value and sensory properties of food are other factors that have an impact on the stress eating relationship. The general trend is that during times of stress, people reduce their intake of healthy, low-fat foods and increase their intake of unhealthy, high fat foods.1 Zellner et al1 reported that when people are under stress, foods that are normally avoided are chosen as people lose control of self-imposed rules. Findings on specific food preferences when a person is stressed are mixed. Sweet foods,15 high fat foods, high protein foods,20 and sweet or bland foods11 have all been preferred in stress-induced eating studies. Consistently, however, there has been an inclination towards “snack” foods as opposed to meal-type foods. Oliver and Wardle16 found that 73.0% of those who increase their food consumption under stress turn to snack foods, such as chocolate biscuits. This relationship seems to be sex dependent.17

Changes in eating behavior and food preference have been widely explored in experimental settings2124 but much less commonly in survey studies, especially among college students. Very little is known about the relationships between dietary habits and stress in the Gulf region countries. Although patchy research on food habits has been conducted in Saudi Arabia,25,26 Oman,27 and Kuwait,10 to the best of our knowledge, no study has explicitly considered the association between food habits and stress in Saudi Arabia. This study was carried out to assess the associations between stress levels and food consumption frequency among students at the College of Applied Medical Sciences of King Saud University. The results of this study may contribute to the improvement of the quality of life of University students.

Methods

A cross-sectional study design was used to collect data from undergraduate students studying at the College of Applied Medical Sciences (CAMS) in Riyadh, Saudi Arabia (SA). The selection of the college was based on its diverse academic programs and students, and it is the largest college in Saudi Arabia. Due to the absence of previous studies on stress among undergraduate students in colleges of allied medical sciences, we carried out a pilot survey of 30 male students and 30 female students using the PSS-10 stress scale. We found stress to be present in 47.0% of males and 59.0% of females. If we assume the power to be set at 95% and 95% confidence intervals, the sample size would need to be 383. Including the 5% increment to compensate for potential nonresponse, the final required sample size was estimated to be 400 students. Since there are two separate colleges, one for males and the other for females, with an equal number of students, we selected 200 random students from each college. None of the students in the sample were found to perform any kind of treatment related to a weight loss goal or dieting.

All students who participated in the study were informed about the nature objectives of the study, and the questionnaire used was explained by well-trained students who acted as research assistants. The students were informed that participation in the study was voluntary and that they had the choice to remain anonymous. Further, a written informed consent was obtained from each participant after explaining the purpose of the study. Approval for conducting the study was obtained from the Research and Ethics Committee of the CAMS, King Saud University.

The questionnaire consisted of three parts: demographic information, Cohen’s Perceived Stress Scale (PSS-10) questionnaire,28 and a dietary intake questionnaire. The demographic information mainly included variables pertaining to the demographic profile and personal factors of the participants, which mainly included age, sex, body weight, family income, living situation, credit hours, and level of study.

Stress Assessment

Stress was assessed using the PSS-10 questionnaire, which is a reliable and valid assessment instrument for the study of stress in college students.29 Cohen30 reported an alpha reliability of 0.78. The PSS has been used in various clinical circumstances, settings and cultures and has been translated into 17 languages, including Arabic.31 The score ranges from 0 to 40 and serves as an outcome variable. The answers were graded on a 5-point Likert scale: never = 0, almost never = 1, sometimes = 2, fairly often = 3, or very often = 4. The positively framed questions, including questions 4, 5, 7, and 8, were reverse scored: never = 4 to very often = 0.

The scores were summed across all scale items, with higher scores indicating higher perceived stress. According to the table for the PSS published by Cohen, a score of 14.2 with an (SD = 6.2) was reported as the average for the 18 to 29-year age group. A cut-off score of 14 for the PSS was chosen, below which students were considered to have a nonsignificant level of stress. A low stress level was determined if scores ranged from 0–13. Scores ranging from 14–26 were considered to correspond to moderate stress, and scores ranging from 27–40 indicated high stress. In this study, the internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha) for the total scale was 0.83. In other related studies, Cronbach’s alpha of the PSS was 0.85 in Germany, 0.81 in Poland, 0.80 in Bulgaria,32 0.76 in Finland,5 0.47 in Egypt,6 and 0.68 in Oman.27 BMI was calculated from self-reported weight and height.

Assessment of Dietary Intake

Self-reported dietary intake for the participants was assessed using a 7-day food frequency questionnaire (FFQ) comprising variables that measured the consumption of sweets (desserts, chocolate, candy), snacks (potato chips, ice cream/milk shakes, cakes, and muffins), fast food (hamburgers, pizza, French fries and chicken nuggets), fresh fruits, vegetables, meat and chicken, beverages (fruit juice, tea, coffee, soft drinks, flavored milk, and energy drinks), fish/seafood, dairy/dairy products (yogurt, milk and cheese), and cereal/cereal products. The instrument was created to include food groups that are important when studying dietary habits, which is in line with other research studies.33,34 The introductory question “How often do you eat the following foods?” asked participants about the frequency of their usual consumption of each food item separately. Food frequency intake responses ranged from never to several times per day. The FFQ was focused on the frequency of selected food items only, and information on the portion size was not included. The consumption frequency of each group of food in the previous week was divided into five categories: 5 = “several times a day”, 4 = “daily”, 3 = “several times a week”, 2 = “1–4 times a month”, and 1 = “never”. No formal test of validity was undertaken, but the questionnaire was very similar to other food frequency questionnaires that had been validated.

Statistical Analysis

The results from the univariate analyses are presented as the mean and standard deviation and absolute (n) and relative (%) frequencies. The amount of food eaten under stress was assessed using the following response options: much more than usual, more than usual, the same as usual, less than usual, or much less than usual. The binomial test and the McNemar chi-square test were used to analyze food flavor, food type, food preparation and the loss of control overeating when stressed and not stressed for both sexes. Logistic regression was also used to assess the associations between stress and dietary behaviors for male and female students separately. The food categories were treated as outcome (dependent) variables, and stress was treated as the independent variable. The intake levels in each food category were divided into two groups using the median intakes as cutoffs in both sex groups.4 The independent variable (perceived stress) was categorized into three levels: not stressed (treated as the reference category), moderate stress, and high stress. Previous studies have found that stress is associated with various sociodemographic factors.35,36 Therefore, the data were adjusted for potential confounders, including age, marital status, family income, living situation, and body mass index (BMI) using the logistic regression model. The results of the regression analyses are presented as odds ratios that are adjusted for confounding variables. A P value < 0.05 was considered statistically significant. The statistical analysis was performed using Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) version 21.0 computer software (SPSS Inc., Chicago, SPSS for Windows).

Results

The characteristics of the study sample are shown in Table 1. Of the participants, 50.0% (200) were male and 50.0% (200) were female. The mean (SD) age of the participants was 21.5 (SD=2.8) years for males and 21.2 ± 3 years for females. Most of the students were single (89.3%), and only 10.7% were married. However, a significantly higher proportion of males (95.5%) were single compared to the females (83.0%). Regarding family income, approximately 51.0% of the students came from a family with an income between 5000 and 10,000 SR, and approximately one-third of the students came from families with incomes less than 5000 SR. Most of the students (81.3%) were living at home with family, especially females (95.5%), with 18.7% of students living off campus and the rest living on campus (10.0%); living apart from family was more common among males than among females. Approximately (10.0%) of males and (12.5%) of females were fourth year students, while the rest were equally distributed between the other years. At the time of the study, the majority of the students (63.5%) indicated that they were taking 17 to 20 credit hours that semester. The prevalence rates of overweight and obesity were (39.0%) for males and (31.0%) for females, whereas (56.0%) of males and (62.5%) of females were considered of a normal weight. Approximately (59.0%) of all participants were found to suffer from some level of stress, with relatively more females (64.5%) than males (54.0%) exhibiting some level of stress (Z = 2.137, p = 0.032). Approximately 28% of female students suffered from mild stress, and (37.0%) experienced high stress. Among males, (32.0%) were suffering from mild stress and (22.0%) were experiencing high stress.

Table 1 Characteristics of the Study Population

Table 2 presents the amount of food eaten under stress for males and females. Most participants reported that they altered their eating behavior in response to stress (83.0%). Females (68.0%) were more likely than males (49.0%) to report eating more (Z = 4.83, p < 0.001), while the percentage that reported eating less was (23.0%) for females and 31% for males (Z = 1.80, p = 0.072). Males (20.0%) were significantly more likely to report no change compared to females (9.0%) (Z = 2.87, p < 0.004).

Table 2 Amount of Food Eaten Under Stress

Table 3 shows the preference for food flavor by sex. The McNemar chi-square test indicated that there was a significant change in preference for sweet-flavored foods when stressed compared to when not stressed for both males and females (p < 0.001). Significantly more females (82.0%) than males (64.5%) (Z = 5.05; p < 0.001) reported that they preferred sweets under stress. Participants of both sexes indicated that they significantly preferred salty flavors when stressed compared to when not stressed (p < 0.001). No significant difference was observed for the change in flavor preferences between males (74.5%) and females (71.5%) (Z = 1.63; p = 0.499). The binomial test showed that the participants, both males and females, reported to prefer bitter flavors significantly less when stressed. Males (16.5%) reported preferring bitter-flavored foods significantly more than females did (4.0%) (Z = 2.81, p < 0.001). Participants reported preferring savory flavors less when stressed compared to when not stressed (p = 0.002 for males and p = 0.005 for females). The results showed that males preferred savory flavors more (47.5%) than females did (37.5%) (Z = 2.03; p = 0.043).

Table 3 Preferences by Sex for Food Flavor (Sweet, Salty, Bitter, Savory), Food Type and Food Preparation When Stressed Compared to When Not Stressed

Table 4 depicts the preferences for food type and food preparation and loss of control eating by sex. The McNemar test indicated that there was a significant change in preferences related to food type and food preparation and the loss of control eating when stressed compared to when not stressed for both sexes. Females (78.0%) reported preferring snack-type food options significantly more than males (20.5%) did when stressed (Z = 10.5; p < 0.001). Males (79.5%) reported preferring meal-type foods significantly more than females (22.0%) did when stressed (Z = 10.5, p < 0.001). Females reported preferring homemade food significantly (72.0%) more than males (27.5%) when stressed, while males (72.5%) preferred take-out food significantly more than females (28.0%) did when stressed. Both male and female participants reported loss of control eating significantly more when stressed compared to when they were not stressed (p < 0.001). Additionally, females (68.0%) reported loss of control eating significantly more than males (49%) (Z = 3.86; p < 0.001).

Table 4 Food Type Preferences and Loss of Control Eating According Stress Status and Sex

Table 5 displays the logistic regression analysis between stress and the selection of various food groups by sex while controlling for potential confounding factors. Female students who experienced moderate to high levels of stress were more likely to eat more sweets and snacks and drink more beverages than the unstressed students. However, the stressed female students were less likely to consume fruits, vegetables, meat, chicken, fish and seafood. Male students who experienced moderate to high levels of stress were more likely to consume more fast food, meat, chicken, fish and sea food than unstressed students. However, the stressed male students were less likely to consume fruits and vegetables.

Table 5 Logistic Regression for Consumption of Various Foods Associated with Stress Levels by Sex

Discussion

Our findings showed that approximately (59.0%) of the undergraduate students at the CAMS suffered from some level of stress, with significantly more female students (64.5%) suffering from a higher level of stress compared to male students (54.0%). This sex difference may be due to the structures of Saudi Arabian society, considering the role of tradition, culture and religion in the social structure of the country. Conservative social norms and cultural restrictions influence the contexts in which women can be involved. Women require permission from their male guardian and company of close male relatives. Culturally, Saudi males are not as strictly supervised as females. Previous studies among university students in Saudi Arabia and other Arab countries with similar cultures have shown the same pattern. For example, in Kuwait,10 (40.0%) of people were stressed, with more females (44.0%) than males (40.9%) experiencing stress. In Jordon,37 (70.0%) of people were stressed, including (73.0%) of females and (63.0%) of males. In Saudi Arabia,38 stress was (71.9%) of people were stressed, including (77.0%) of females and (64.0%) of males. The prevalence of stress found in this study was higher than the prevalence of stress observed in similar studies conducted in Malaysia39 (36.0%), Turkey (27.0%),35 the USA40 (38.0%) and Hong Kong41 (43.0%) but was lower than the prevalence of stress in Australia42 (83.9%) and Iran43 (61.3%).

The results from this study showed that most of the students altered their eating behavior (increased or decreased consumption) in response to stress. The percentage that reported eating more when stressed (56.0%) was lower than the percentages reported by Weinstein et al14 (57.0%) and Kandiah et al9 (62.0%) and was very close to the percentage reported by Willenbring et al44 (48.0%) but was higher than those reported by Oliver and Wardle16 (42.0%) and Stone and Brawnell45 (28.0%). The percentage that reported eating less when stressed (27.0%) was below those reported by Oliver and Wardle (38.0%)16 and Kandiah et al9 (38.0%). These differences in results may be mainly due to the methods used in those studies. For example, in the Stone and Brownell45 study, participants rated their stress daily and recorded whether they had eaten more, the same as, or less than usual over a period of 84 days. Additionally, in Stallman’s study,42 he sampled from two universities, and the data were collected using both web-based questionnaires and face-to-face interviews. Of the two universities included in his study, one had a very low response rate, which may have created a response bias. In the present study, we sampled from one university and estimated consumption patterns based on the memories of the respondents. We collected data using a self-administered questionnaire. The study also provided preliminary evidence that females may be more prone to alter certain eating patterns in response to stress. This was shown by results that indicated that females were more likely than males to report adjusting their food consumption in response to stress, as approximately (69.0%) of females reported that they increased their food intake and approximately (23.0%) reported that they decreased their food intake when stressed. Males reported a (44.0%) increase in food intake and an approximate (31.0%) decrease in food intake.

Stress was associated with reported increased preferences for salty flavors, but these results contradicted the results of the study by Grunberg and Straub,11 in which the participants choose sweet or bland foods over salty foods during stressful moments. This change in flavor preference was not associated with sex (males 74.5%, females 76.5%). The effect may be a product of sympatho-adrenal medullary activity (increased renal sodium excretion may cause a sodium deficiency and increase the appetite for sodium).46 Respondents also preferred bitter and savory foods less when stressed than when they were not stressed. This decreased preference for bitter foods may be a possible reflection of stress driving flavor preference to more palatable foods.16,47 In addition, reports of both bitter and savory flavor preferences appeared to be influenced by sex. When stressed, males reported preferring bitter (males 11.5%, females 4.0%) and savory (males 47.5%, females 37.5%) flavors more than females did.

According to this study, sex influences preferences for the method of food preparation. Male students reported a preference for take-out food (68.0%), while female students (72.0%) reported preparing their own food when stressed. Previous research has shown that the reason participants choose the food they consume when stressed is convenience and taste.13,16 During stress, time and energy are not plentiful; and thus, convenient foods are preferable to foods that require more time and energy to prepare. For females, going out to a restaurant may be too time consuming during stressful moments when time is limited. This is because conservative social norms and cultural restrictions influence the context in which females can go out. Thus, females prefer to prepare their own food rather than ordering in or having someone else prepare their food; this difference may also be because stress is more frequently experienced when at home and alone. The majority of female students were living at home with their parents (95.5%). As another explanation, female participants may prefer to prepare their own food because they are seeking specific flavors, tastes or nutritional components that are depleted during stress or that encourage positive effects. Males are not strictly supervised, and they can freely go out. This might explain the clear sex differences in food preparation behavior when stressed.

During stressful moments, more females (78.0%) than males (20.5%) tended to prefer snack-related foods (e.g., sweets, chocolate, ice cream, cookies), while males tended to prefer meal-related foods (e.g., meat, fish). This corresponds well with the results of Wansink et al,17 which demonstrated that a higher percentage of females preferred candy and chocolate (69.0% female, 58.0% male), but a lower percentage of females preferred meal-related foods such as meat and sea food (52.0% female, 77.0% male). Oliver and Wardle,16 however, did not find a sex difference in snacking behavior under stress. Snacks may be preferred during stress because of their typical high fat and sugar content48 and because they are easy to prepare. Snacking during stress may be beneficial as a coping mechanism, as energy-rich snacks in the afternoon have been shown to increase attention and memory.48 When stressed, 69.5% of females preferred small and bite-sized food compared to only 21.0% of males. Oliver and Wardle16 explained that the preference for small and bite-sized foods may validate a preference for snack-type foods because they are more easily digested while gut activity is suppressed and may additionally indicate the ability to eat while completing a secondary activity (such as studying) or engaging in a distraction (such as watching TV).

The relationship between stress and food selection patterns by sex was examined using logistic regression after adjusting for socioeconomic variables. The results revealed that both males and females were more likely to consume unhealthy foods under stress compared to when they were not stressed. The consumption of unhealthy foods was higher among female students. Stressed females were 3.06, 2.66, and 1.97 times more likely to consume sweets, snacks and beverages, respectively, compared to unstressed students. The preference for unhealthy food is evident in the literature.1,9,11,49 Females tend to be more health-conscious and tend to go on more diets than males;50 thus, they are more susceptible to the lack of restraint involved in stress eating. Therefore, females tend to allow themselves to consume these unhealthy high-calorie foods when the inhibition they usually show is reduced by stress.1 Moreover, sex dimorphisms in the functioning of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis have been documented; when the HPA is activated, glucocorticoid (GC) output is thought to be higher among females than among males.51 For male students, consumption of fast food, meat, fish and sea food was more common when stressed. Stressed males were 2.87, 2.31, and 1.88 times more likely to consume fast food, meat, fish and sea food, respectively, compared to unstressed students. Similar results were reported among university/college students from the USA,52 Bangladesh,53 Lebanon54 and Italy.55 Females and males also ate fewer fruits and vegetables when stressed. Stressed females were 51.0% and 54.0% less likely to consume fruits and vegetables, respectively, compared to unstressed students, while stressed males were 38.0% and 34.0% less likely to consume fruits and vegetables, respectively, compared to unstressed students. Similar results were reported among university students in the USA,52 Bangladesh,53 China,56 and Greece.57 Similarly, the consumption of cereal/cereal products and dairy/dairy products was more common among male students. This finding was in accordance with the results of other studies.4,58

Findings on specific food preferences when a person is stressed are mixed. Two reasons might account for specific food preference during stress. When under stress, foods that one normally avoids are chosen as people lose control of self-imposed rules, which is a further confirmation of the role of disinhibition in stress eating.1,16 Kandiah 20069 speculated that the food composition (high fat/energy content) rather than aesthetic properties (taste) could have more influence on why certain foods are chosen over others during stressful times. Keren 20154 attributed the preference of these foods to the quick availability and palatability of these foods. The second reason that an individual chooses the foods they do during stress is the influence of the hormone cortisol on the metabolism. Not only has cortisol been shown to regulate energy balance through glucogenesis and lipolysis, but cortisol also influences macronutrient selection (increased appetite for carbohydrates and for fat) in rodents.24,59

This study had some limitations. The study was cross-sectional, and we could not reach a conclusion on the direction of the association between food selection and stress on the basis of our results. We used an FFQ and, as a prior investigation explained, FFQs can lead to underestimating dietary consumption.60 We examined the relationship between food selection and stress in one university and one college in Riyadh due to the difficulty in securing permission from other universities. We recommend that this relationship be further examined among different universities.

Nevertheless, this study was the first to examine the association between food selection and stress among undergraduate university students in Saudi Arabia by sex while controlling for other confounding variables. Additionally, the response rate was high. All students responded fully to the questionnaires. We measured a wide range of eating behavior variables, providing a comprehensive assessment of the correlation between stress and food selection among college students. We did not compare overeaters to undereaters, which would help to elucidate the association between stress and eating habits.

In conclusion, the results support the notion that both males and females perceive that stress leads to unhealthy changes in eating patterns. The present investigation also provides preliminary evidence suggesting that females perceived themselves to be more susceptible than males to stress-related changes in food consumption, especially unhealthy changes. Females were more likely to consume sweets, snacks and beverages and fewer fruits and vegetables, while males were more likely to consume meat and fast foods. Given that women are at increased risk for disordered eating,61 it would be sensible to continue to explore the unique susceptibility to unhealthy changes in stress-eating patterns, such as consuming sweets and snacks, among females to further elucidate how and why sex impacts some stress eating behaviors.

Data Sharing Statement

The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Acknowledgments

The authors extend their appreciation to the Deanship of Scientific Research at King Saud University for funding this work through research group No (RG- 1439-81).

Author contributions

All authors contributed to data analysis, drafting and revising the article, gave final approval of the version to be published, and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

Disclosure

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

References

1. Zellner DA, Loaiza S, Gonzalez Z, et al. Food selection changes under stress. Physiol Behav. 2006;87:789–793. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.01.014

2. Cohen S, Janicki-Deverts D, Miller GE. Psychological stress and disease. JAMA. 2007;298:1685–1687. doi:10.1001/jama.298.14.1685

3. Mikolajczyk RT, El Ansari W, Maxwell AE. Food consumption frequency and perceived stress and depressive symptoms among students in three European countries. Nutr J. 2009;8:31. doi:10.1186/1475-2891-8-31

4. Keren P, Faruk A, Patricia L, Juliet W. Stress and dietary behavior among first-year university students in Australia: sex differences. Nutrition. 2015;31:324–330. doi:10.1016/j.nut.2014.08.004

5. El-Ansari W, Sakari S, Gabriele BB. Mood and food at the University of Turku in Finland: nutritional correlates of perceived stress are most pronounced among overweight students. Int J Public Health. 2015;60:707–716. doi:10.1007/s00038-015-0717-4

6. El-Ansari W, Gabriele BB. Nutritional Correlates of Perceived Stress among University Students in Egypt. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2015;12:14164–14176. doi:10.3390/ijerph121114164

7. Liu C, Xie B, Chou CP, et al. Perceived stress, depression and food consumption frequency in the college students of China seven cities. Physiol Behav. 2007;92:748–754. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.05.068

8. Axelson ML. The impact of culture on food-related behavior. Ann Rev Nutr. 1986;6:345–363. doi:10.1146/annurev.nu.06.070186.002021

9. Kandiah J, Yake M, Jones J, Meyer M. Stress influences appetite and comfort food preferences in college women. Nutr Res. 2006;26:118–123. doi:10.1016/j.nutres.2005.11.010

10. Ahmed F, Al-Radhwan L, Al-Azmi GZS, Al-Beajan M. Association between stress and dietary behaviors among undergraduate students in Kuwait: gender differences. J Nutr Health Sci. 2014;1:1–8.

11. Grunberg NE, Straub RO. The role of gender and taste class in the effects of stress on eating. Health Psychol. 1992;11:97–100. doi:10.1037/0278-6133.11.2.97

12. O’Connor DB, Jones F, Coner M, McMillan B, Ferguson E. Effects of daily hassles and eating style on eating behavior. Health Psychol. 2008;27:S20–S31. doi:10.1037/0278-6133.27.1.S20

13. Pollard TM, Steptoe A, Canaan L, Davies GJ, Wardle J. Effects of academic examination stress on eating behavior and blood lipid levels. Int J Behav Med. 1995;2:299. doi:10.1207/s15327558ijbm0204_2

14. Weinstein SE, Shide DJ, Rolls BJ. Changes in food intake in response to stress in men and women: psychological factors. Appetite. 1997;28:7–18. doi:10.1006/appe.1996.0056

15. Oliver G, Wardle J, Gibson L. Stress and food choice: a laboratory study. Psychosom Med. 2000;62:853–865. doi:10.1097/00006842-200011000-00016

16. Oliver G, Wardle J. Perceived effects of stress on food choice. Physiol Behav. 1999;66:511–515. doi:10.1016/S0031-9384(98)00322-9

17. Wansink B, Cheney MM, Chan N. Exploring comfort food preferences across age and gender. Physiol Behav. 2003;79:739–747. doi:10.1016/S0031-9384(03)00203-8

18. Wardle J, Steptoe A, Oliver G, Lipsey Z. Stress, dietary restraint and food intake. J Psychosom Res. 2000;48:195–202. doi:10.1016/S0022-3999(00)00076-3

19. Rand CS, Macgregor AM. Successful weight loss following obesity surgery and the perceived liability of morbid obesity. Int J Obes. 1991;15:577–579.

20. Patel KA, Schlundt DG. Impact of moods and social context on eating behavior. Appetite. 2001;36:111–118. doi:10.1006/appe.2000.0385

21. Heatherton TF, Herman CP, Polivy J. Effects of physical threat and ego threat on eating behavior. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1991;60:138–143. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.60.1.138

22. Herman CP, Polivy J, Lank CN, Heatherton TF. Anxiety, hunger, and eating behavior. J Abnorm Psychol. 1987;96:264–269. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.96.3.264

23. Schotte DE, Cools J, McNally RJ. Film-induced negative affect triggers overeating in restrained eaters. J Abnorm Psychol. 1990;99(3):317–320. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.99.3.317

24. Epel E, Lapidus R, McEwen B, Brownell K. Stress may add bite to appetite in women: a laboratory study of stress-induced cortisol and eating behavior. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2001;26:37–49. doi:10.1016/S0306-4530(00)00035-4

25. El Hamid Hussein RA. Socioeconomic status and dietary habits as predictors of home breakfast skipping in young women. J Egypt Public Health Assoc. 2014;89:100–104. doi:10.1097/01.EPX.0000452288.49308.40

26. AL-Qauhiz NM. Obesity among Saudi Female University Students: dietary habits and health behaviors. J Egypt Public Health Assoc. 2010;85:45–59.

27. Kusum Lata Mishra GPM. Establishing relationship between “Stress” and “Eating” leading to overweight among college students in sultanate of Oman. J Bus Econ. 2013;4:529–535.

28. Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R. A global measure of perceived stress. J Health Soc Behav. 1983;24:385–396. doi:10.2307/2136404

29. Roberti JW, Harrington LN, Storch EA. Further psychometric support for the 10-item version of the perceived stress scale. J Coll Couns. 2006;9:135–147. doi:10.1002/j.2161-1882.2006.tb00100.x

30. Cohen S, Williamson G. Perceived stress in a probability sample of the United States. In: Spacapan S, Oskamp S, editors. The Social Psychology of Health. Newbury Park, CA: Sage; 1988.

31. Chaaya M, Osman H, Naassan G, Mahfoud Z. Validation of the Arabic version of the cohen Perceived Stress Scale (PSS-10) among pregnant and postpartum women. BMC Psychiatry. 2010;10:111. doi:10.1186/1471-244X-10-111

32. Mikolajczyk RT, Maxwell AE, Naydenova V, Meier S, El Ansari W. Depressive symptoms and perceived burdens related to being a student: survey in three European countries. Clin Pract Epidemiol Ment Health. 2008;4:19. doi:10.1186/1745-0179-4-19

33. Osler M, Heitmann BL. The validity of a short food frequency questionnaire and its ability to measure changes in food intake: a longitudinal study. Int J Epidemiol. 1996;25:1023–1029. doi:10.1093/ije/25.5.1023

34. Roddam AW, Spencer E, Banks E, et al. Reproducibility of a short semi-quantitative food group questionnaire and its performance in estimating nutrient intake compared with a 7-day diet diary in the Million Women Study. Public Health Nutr. 2005;8:201–213. doi:10.1079/PHN2004676

35. Bayram N, Bilgel N. The prevalence and socio-demographic correlations of depression, anxiety and stress among a group of university students. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2008;43:667–672. doi:10.1007/s00127-008-0345-x

36. Cvetkovski S, Reavley NJ, Jorm AF. The prevalence and correlates of psychological distress in Australian tertiary students compared with their community peers. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2012;46:457–467. doi:10.1177/0004867411435290

37. Abu-Ghazaleh SB, Rajab LD, Sonbol HN. Psychological stress among dental students at the University of Jordan. J Dent Educ. 2011;75:1107–1114.

38. Hamza MA, Abdulghani AA, AlKanhal ES, Mahmoud Gominda GP, Eiad AA. Stress and its effects on medical students: a cross-sectional study at a college of medicine in Saudi Arabia. J Health Popul Nutr. 2011;29(5):516–522.

39. Gan WY, Nasir MM, Zalilah MS, Hazizi AS. Disordered eating behaviors, depression, anxiety and stress among Malaysian university students. Coll Stud J. 2011;45(2):296–309.

40. Vanessa LE, Keryn EP, Cheryl LP. Perceived stress and dietary choices: the moderating role of stress management. Eat Behav. 2016;22:211–216. doi:10.1016/j.eatbeh.2016.06.008

41. Wong JG, Cheung EP, Chan KK, Ma KK, Tang SW. Web-based survey of depression, anxiety and stress in first-year tertiary education students in Hong Kong. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2006;40(9):777–782. doi:10.1080/j.1440-1614.2006.01883.x

42. Stallman HM. Psychological distress in university students: a comparison with general population data. Aust Psychol. 2010;45:249–257. doi:10.1080/00050067.2010.482109

43. Koochaki GM, Charkazi A, Hasanzadeh A, Saedani M, Qorbani M, Marjani A. Prevalence of stress among Iranian medical students: a questionnaire survey. East Mediterr Health J. 2011;17:593–598. doi:10.26719/2011.17.7.593

44. Willenbring M, Levine A, Morley J. Stress induced eating and food preference in humans: a pilot study. Int J Eat Disord. 1986;5(5):855–864. doi:10.1002/(ISSN)1098-108X

45. Stone A, Brownell KD. The stress-eating paradox: multiple daily measurements in adult males and females. Psychol Health. 1994;9:425–436. doi:10.1080/08870449408407469

46. Torres SJ, Turner AI, Nowson CA. Does stress induce salt intake? Br J Nutr. 2010;103:1562–1568. doi:10.1017/S000711451000098X

47. Tryon M. Stress “and” food “intake:” what’s “the deal” “with your” meal. CAB reviews: perspective in agriculture, veterinary science. Nutr Nat Resour. 2011;6:34.

48. Kanarek R. Psychological effects of snacks and altered meal frequency. Br J Nutr. 1997;77:S105–S118. doi:10.1079/BJN19970108

49. Greeno CG, Wing RR. Stress-induced eating. Psychol Bull. 1994;115:444–464. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.115.3.444

50. Davis C, Shapiro CM, Elliott S, Dionne M. Personality and other correlates of dietary restraint: an age by sex comparison. Pers Individ Dif. 1993;14(2):297–305. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(93)90127-O

51. Kudielka BM, Kirschbaum C, Schotte DE, Cools J, McNally RJ. Sex differences in HPA axis responses to stress: a review. Biol Psychol. 2005;69:113–132. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2004.11.009

52. Stockton S, Baker D. College students’ perceptions of fast food restaurant menu items on health. Am J Health Educ. 2013;44(2):74–80. doi:10.1080/19325037.2013.764242

53. Goon S, Bipasha MS, Islam MS. Fast food consumption and obesity risk among university students of Bangladesh. EJPM. 2014;2(6):99–104. doi:10.11648/j.ejpm.20140206.14

54. El-Kassas G, Itani L, El Ali Z. Obesity risk factors among Beirut Arab University students in Tripoli-Lebanon. J Nutr Food Sci. 2015;5:421.

55. Teleman AA, de Waure C, Soffiani V, Poscia A, Di Pietro ML. Nutritional habits in Italian university students. Ann Ist Super Sanita. 2015;51(2):99–105. doi:10.4415/ANN_15_02_05

56. Rui-Hui M. Fast-food consumption among college students based on cost and thermal analysis. Adv J Food Sci Technol. 2015;8(2):122–125. doi:10.19026/ajfst.8.1478

57. Evagelou E, Vlachou E, Polikandrioti M, Koutelekos I, Dousis E, Kyritsi E. Exploration of nursing students’ dietary habits. Health Sci J. 2014;8(4):452–468.

58. Mogre V, Nyaba R, Aleyira S, Sam NB. Demographic, dietary and physical activity predictors of general and abdominal obesity among university students: a cross-sectional study. Springerplus. 2015;4:226. doi:10.1186/s40064-015-0999-2

59. Tempel DL, Leibowitz SF. Adrenal steroid receptors: interactions with brain neuropeptide systems in relation to nutrient intake and metabolism. J Neuroendocrinol. 1994;6:479–501. doi:10.1111/jne.1994.6.issue-5

60. Hu FB, Rimm E, Smith-Warner SA, et al. Reproducibility and validity of dietary patterns assessed with a food-frequency questionnaire. Am J Clin Nutr. 1999;69:243–249. doi:10.1093/ajcn/69.2.243

61. Klein L, Faraday M, Grunberg N. Gender differences in eating after exposure to a noise stressor. Ann Behav Med. 1996;18:S103.

Creative Commons License © 2020 The Author(s). This work is published and licensed by Dove Medical Press Limited. The full terms of this license are available at https://www.dovepress.com/terms.php and incorporate the Creative Commons Attribution - Non Commercial (unported, v3.0) License. By accessing the work you hereby accept the Terms. Non-commercial uses of the work are permitted without any further permission from Dove Medical Press Limited, provided the work is properly attributed. For permission for commercial use of this work, please see paragraphs 4.2 and 5 of our Terms.