Back to Journals » Psychology Research and Behavior Management » Volume 16

Effect of Social Media Addiction on Executive Functioning Among Young Adults: The Mediating Roles of Emotional Disturbance and Sleep Quality

Authors Zhang K, Li P, Zhao Y, Griffiths MD , Wang J, Zhang MX

Received 12 April 2023

Accepted for publication 17 May 2023

Published 25 May 2023 Volume 2023:16 Pages 1911—1920

DOI https://doi.org/10.2147/PRBM.S414625

Checked for plagiarism Yes

Review by Single anonymous peer review

Peer reviewer comments 2

Editor who approved publication: Dr Igor Elman



Kuo Zhang,1 Peiyu Li,1 Ying Zhao,2 Mark D Griffiths,3 Jingxin Wang,4 Meng Xuan Zhang5

1Department of Social Psychology, Zhou Enlai School of Government, Nankai University, Tianjin, 300350, People’s Republic of China; 2Mental Health Education Center, Yangzhou University, Yangzhou, Jiangsu, 225009, People’s Republic of China; 3International Gaming Research Unit, Psychology Department, Nottingham Trent University, Nottingham, NG1 4FQ, UK; 4Academy of Psychology and Behavior, Faculty of Psychology, Tianjin Normal University, Tianjin, People’s Republic of China; 5Department of Medical Humanities, School of Humanities, Southeast University, Nanjing, Jiangsu, 211189, People’s Republic of China

Correspondence: Meng Xuan Zhang, Department of Medical Humanities, School of Humanities, Southeast University, Nanjing, Jiangsu, 211189, People’s Republic of China, Email [email protected]

Introduction: The increased research examining social media addiction with its negative consequences has raised concerns over the past decade. However, little research has investigated the association between social media addiction and executive functioning as well as the mechanisms underlying this relationship.
Methods: Using a survey, the present study examined the association between social media addiction and executive functioning via emotional disturbance and sleep quality among 1051 Chinese young adults, aged 18 to 27 years old (M=21.02 years [SD=1.89]; 34.41% male).
Results: The results showed that social media addiction had a significant negative association with executive functioning but positive associations with emotional disturbance and poor sleep quality. Structural equation modeling suggested that there was a significant direct effect between social media addiction and executive functioning. Indirect effects via two paths (ie, emotional disturbance alone, and both emotional disturbance and sleep quality) were also statistically significant.
Discussion: The findings indicate that both emotional disturbance and poor sleep quality are risk-enhancing mediators in the relationship between social media addiction and executive functioning. Intervention programs (eg, emotional regulation strategies) should be considered to reduce the adverse effects of social media addiction on cognitive impairment among young adults.

Keywords: social media addiction, executive functioning, emotional disturbance, sleep quality, young adults

Introduction

The development of mobile internet technology and the availability of mobile devices such as smartphones/tablets has resulted in a reliance on the use of such technologies. The smartphone has become an indispensable part of people’s daily lives as they are used to interacting with others, maintaining relationships, and dealing with work issues. Smartphone use is inextricably linked with social media use1 and people’s use of social networking sites [eg, Facebook, Twitter, WeChat, TikTok].2,3 The number of social media users has continued to rise worldwide.4 In China [where the present study was carried out), there are nearly a billion social media users according to the 48th report of China Internet Network Information Center.5

In terms of patterns of social media use, active use and passive use are common. Active use refers to direct exchanges with others on social media (eg, texting, posting, and commenting), whereas passive use refers to browsing others’ online life without any linking and communications.6 Previous research found passive use predicted a decline in emotional well-being, while active use did not.6 However, A review found most research did not support that conclusion.7 Although social media use may provide many benefits in modern society, excessive or problematic use of social media can have adverse consequences on physical and mental health among a minority of individuals, especially young adults.8–11 For some individuals, this may result in problematic social media use and (in extreme cases) social media addiction (SMA).1

SMA has been commonly viewed as a type of behavioral addiction12,13, although it was not formally recognized in the fifth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5)14. SMA has been defined as a problematic pattern of social media use with uncontrollable urges, and the inability to successfully regulate the use of social media resulting in adverse consequences on relationships, occupation, and/or education.15 In extreme cases, the behavior results in consequences associated with other more traditional addictions such as salience, tolerance, withdrawal symptoms, relapse, mood modification, and conflict.16

Research has shown that problematic social media use has a relatively high prevalence and significant harm to health and well-being,3,17 which has gradually emerged as a problem of global concern. The prevalence of problematic social media use appears to be high in China, especially among young adults. One study reported that 44.9% of Chinese university students aged over 18 years old (N=1090) were at risk of SMA.17 Moreover, negative consequences have been associated with SMA, such as depression and anxiety,18 burnout,19 and poor sleep quality.20 To date, most research has focused on the negative impacts of SMA in relation to emotional and mental health. Few studies have examined its harmful effects on cognitive abilities or the underlying mechanisms in such relationships. Therefore, the present study examined the effect of SMA on executive functioning as well as the mechanisms of emotional disturbance and sleep quality among Chinese young adults.

Executive functions are higher-order cognitive abilities/processes tied to the frontal lobes of the brain, which encompass abilities such as planning, organization, problem-solving, working memory, and decision-making to facilitate new ways of optimizing behavior in day-to-day environments.21,22 From the cognitive neuroscience perspective, executive functions involve top-down control in the prefrontal cortex related to reward and emotion, and addictive behaviors may permanently change the neural network and damage prefrontal cortex functioning, such as top-down control.23,24 Executive dysfunction (impairment of executive functioning) has been increasingly recognized among individuals experiencing addiction.25

For internet use (including social media use), a review suggested that information technology can place a burden on cognitive processes, including executive functioning, even for individuals not diagnosed with internet use disorders.26 A meta-analysis also reported that problematic internet use was associated with executive functioning impairment, such as inhibitory control, decision-making, and working memory.27 Moreover, previous research has found that executive functioning is negatively associated with problematic social media use.28 Inhibitory control, one of the processes in executive functioning, was found to be negatively associated with SMA among Chinese female university students.29 Therefore, the present study aimed to examine the relationship between general executive functioning and SMA. In addition to its direct effect, it also investigated the indirect effects of SMA, via emotional disturbance and sleep quality, on general executive functioning, which has implications for further intervention.

Emotional disturbance attributes negative consequences among young adults, including SMA. For example, in previous studies, SMA has been positively associated with emotional disturbance, such as depression, anxiety, and stress.30,31 Individuals with higher severity of SMA have reported greater symptoms of emotional disturbance.32 Moreover, SMA has also been associated with difficulties in emotional regulation,33 which could contribute to emotional disturbance. Furthermore, the harm of emotional disturbance on the processes and abilities of executive functioning has also been reported in previous research. For example, a longitudinal study found that depression and generalized anxiety symptoms were related to poorer executive functioning capacity 18 years later.34 An experimental study reported that central executive functioning on a nonverbal task was impaired by trait anxiety.35 Moreover, anxiety has been found to have adverse effects on the processing efficiency of two central executive functioning related to attentional control: inhibition and shifting.36 Additionally, depressive rumination has been associated with impairments in inhibition.37

Sleep quality is an important predictor of health and well-being. Individuals with SMA have the risk of poor sleep quality, including difficulty in falling asleep and/or maintaining sleep.32 One study found that only social media use at bedtime was associated with a higher risk of poor sleep quality among freshman college students.38 Another study with an adult sample aged 18 to 58 years old found that problematic social media use was related to poor sleep quality.39 In the present study, individuals with a higher level of SMA were expected to have poorer sleep quality. Moreover, in the relationship between sleep quality and executive functioning, previous research found insufficient sleep leads to poor self-regulatory capacity, and individuals with less sleep are unable to recover from the depletion of self-control resources.40 For example, individuals with poorer sleep report poorer executive functioning than good sleepers.41 Another experimental study utilizing event-related potential demonstrated that sleep deprivation led to poorer executive functioning among Chinese males.42 Sleep quality has also been demonstrated to be a mediator between problematic social media use and cognitive failures (eg, memory and motor functioning).39

Previous research has reported that both emotional disturbance and sleep quality are related to behavioral addictions, including SMA.32,43,44 Moreover, the emotional disturbance was also associated with sleep quality. For example, one study showed that high levels of depression, anxiety, and stress decreased students’ sleep quality.45 Another study found that poor sleep quality was negatively related to psychological well-being among Chinese undergraduates.46 However, prior to the present study, no research has explored the serial mediation of emotional disturbance and sleep quality between the relationship between SMA and executive functioning.

The present study addresses this research gap and empirically tested the effects of SMA on general executive functioning and the potential underlying mechanisms of emotional disturbance and sleep quality among Chinese young adults. Using structural equation modeling (SEM), the following hypotheses were tested:

H1: Executive functioning will have a negative association with SMA.

H2: Emotional disturbance will have a negative relationship with executive functioning.

H3: Emotional disturbance will mediate the relationship between SMA and executive functioning.

H4: Sleep quality will be positively related to executive functioning.

H5: Sleep quality will mediate between SMA and executive functioning.

Methods

Participants and Procedure

The data were collected from four public universities in Tianjin and Henan provinces (China) via convenience sampling (June-September 2019). Students were invited to participate in class groups using WeChat (a Chinese social media application). Participants were provided with a link to the online survey on a Chinese web survey platform (wjx.cn). Before completing the survey, all the participants gave their consent and knew their rights to participation (eg, withdrawal at any time without penalty). Undergraduate students (N=1051) voluntarily completed the self-report survey without any remuneration. The age ranged from 18 to 27 years old (Mage=21.02 years [SD=1.89]; 34.41% male). Ethics approval was obtained from the first author’s departmental Ethics Committee.

Measures

Social Media Addiction

SMA was assessed using the Bergen Social Media Addiction Scale (BSMAS),47 which was modified from the Bergen Facebook Addiction Scale.48 The scale contains six items reflecting the core elements of addiction (ie, salience, conflict, mood modification, withdrawal, tolerance, and relapse).16 The items concern experiences during the past year (eg, “Felt an urge to use social media more and more”) and are answered on a five-point scale, ranging from 1 (very rarely) to 5 (very often). The total score was calculated and higher scores indicate a greater risk of SMA. The internal consistency reliability of BSMAS was good in the present study (α =0.84).

Emotional Disturbance

The emotional disturbance was assessed using the 21-item Depression Anxiety Stress Scale (DASS-21).49 The DASS-21 comprises three subscales: depression, anxiety, and stress. Participants respond to how often they experienced such symptoms (eg, “I couldn’t seem to experience any positive feeling at all”) over the past week on a four-point Likert scale ranging from 0 (never) to 3 (most of the time) and higher scores mean a higher level of distress. The internal consistency reliability of the DASS in the present study was excellent (α=0.94) while the alpha coefficients of the subscales of depression, anxiety, and stress were 0.87, 0.83, and 0.84, respectively.

Sleep Quality

Five items from the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI)50 were used to assess sleep quality. Participants responded these items (eg, “Wake up in the middle of the night or early morning”) on a four-point scale ranging from 0 (never) to 3 (above 3 times per week). Higher scores indicate greater sleep problems and poorer sleep quality. The internal consistency reliability of this scale was very good in the present study (α=0.85).

Executive Functioning

Executive functioning was assessed using Webexec, a six-item scale.51 Items (eg, “Do you find it difficult to keep your attention on a particular task?”) are rated on a four-point scale ranging from 1 (no problems experienced) to 4 (a great many problems experienced). All the scores of items were reversed and the total score of this scale was calculated with a higher score indicating better executive functioning. The internal consistency reliability of the scale was very good in the present study (α=0.88).

In relation to demographic information, participants also provided their gender (male=1; female=2) and age (in years) in the survey.

Data Analysis

Data analyses were conducted using SPSS 22.0 and Amos 24.0. First, descriptive statistics and Pearson correlations were conducted among the main variables and demographics. Second, structural equation modeling (SEM) analysis was performed with SMA as the independent variable, mental distress, and sleep quality as the mediator variables, and executive functioning as the dependent variable. Item parceling rather than individual items was used in the measurement model to reduce the risk of convergence problems and improve model fits.52 The SEM utilized maximum likelihood (ML) estimation in the AMOS 24.0 program. According to the suggestion of Kline,53 the following indicators were used to evaluate the model fit in the present study: χ2/df ratio (≤ 3), Comparative Fit Index (CFI; ≥ 0.95), Tucker–Lewis Index (TLI; ≥ 0.95), Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA; ≤ 0.05), and Standardized Root Mean Square Residual (SRMR; ≤ 0.05). Finally, the multi-mediation analyses were conducted using the PROCESS macro in SPSS 22.0.54,55

Results

Preliminary Analyses

Descriptive statistics (ie, means and standard deviations), and Pearson’s bivariate correlational coefficients for all observed variables are shown in Table 1. For demographic variables, gender was only significantly associated with SMA (r=0.10, p<0.001), with females reporting higher levels of SMA than males. No gender and age effects were observed in relation to executive functioning, emotional disturbance, or sleep quality (ps > 0.05).

Table 1 Descriptive Statistics and Correlations Among Variables

The results of Pearson correlation analyses showed that SMA had a significant negative association with executive functioning (r=−0.38, p<0.001), supporting H1. There were positive associations between SMA and emotional disturbance (r=0.39, p<0.001) as well as sleep quality (r=0.31, p<0.001). Executive functioning was significantly and negatively correlated with emotional disturbance (r=−0.77, p<0.001), and (poor) sleep quality (r=−0.62, p<0.001) which supported H2 and H4.

Structural Equation Model

The hypothesized model, in which SMA was expected to exert not only a direct effect on executive functioning but also an indirect effect on emotional disturbance and sleep quality, was tested first. Gender was added as a control variable because of its significant correlation with SMA. The fit indices of this model were good, χ2(82)=203.967, χ2/df=2.49, CFI= 0.986, TLI= 0.982, RMSEA= 0.038, 90% CI [0.031, 0.044], and SRMR= 0.028. All the factor loadings for the indicators of the latent variables were significant (p<0.001), demonstrating that the latent constructs were well represented by their indicators. Furthermore, tests of parameter estimates showed that all direct path coefficients were significant in the proposed directions, except for the path from SMA to sleep quality. These results suggested that emotional disturbance and sleep quality may play a partial mediating role in the relationship between SMA and executive functioning.

Full versus Partial Mediation

To test the assumption that emotional disturbance and sleep quality mediate the relationship between SMA and executive functioning, the following two potential mediation models were compared using a chi-square difference test: (i) a full mediation model with the direct path from SMA to executive functioning constrained to zero; (ii) a partial mediation model with the above direct path not constrained. The chi-square difference test showed that after removing the above direct path, the fit of the model was significantly reduced (Δχ2 (1, N=1051) =12.164, p<0.001, ΔCFI=0.002). Therefore, the partial mediation model was supported. The final model is shown in Figure 1. Except for the path from SMA and sleep quality (β=0.06, p=0.06), all other path coefficients were significant in the model, among which SMA, emotional disturbance, and sleep quality significantly, negatively predicted executive functioning (β=−0.09, −0.68, and −0.18, ps<0.001).

Figure 1 The SEM regarding the mediation effects of emotional disturbance and sleep quality.

Notes: The path coefficients are standardized. Psq1, Psq2= 1st, 2nd items parcel for poor sleep quality. Ef1, Ef2, Ef3= 1st, 2nd, 3rd items parcel for executive functioning. ***p<0.001. Sleep quality: Higher scores indicate poorer sleep quality.

Assessment of Mediation

Direct, indirect, and total effects are shown in Table 2. The direct effect of SMA on executive functioning was statistically significant (β=−0.09, 95% CI [−0.13, −0.05]). The indirect effects of SMA on executive functioning via emotional disturbance alone (β=−0.24, 95% CI [−0.28, −0.20]) and both emotional disturbance and sleep quality (β=−0.04, 95% CI [−0.06, −0.03]), respectively, were also found significant, which supported H3 and H6.

Table 2 Direct Effect, and Indirect Effects, and Total Effect of the Pathways Tested

However, there was no significant indirect effect on the relationship of SMA➔ sleep quality ➔ executive functioning (p >0.05), therefore H5 was not supported. The mediating effect of SMA on executive functioning accounted for 76% of the total effect. This partial mediation model explained 20%, 60%, and 76% of the variances in emotional disturbance, sleep quality, and executive functioning.

Discussion

SMA with its negative consequences has become a matter of public concern in recent years. The present study found a higher level of SMA was related to greater severity of emotional disturbance, poorer sleep quality, and poorer executive functioning, which suggests that the adverse effects of SMA not only on mental health but also on cognitive functions.

Supporting H1, a negative association was found between SMA and executive functioning, which is consistent with previous findings in investigating the relationships between addictive behaviors (eg, Internet gaming disorder and Internet addiction) and executive functioning.56,57 The results of SEM suggested a significant direct effect of SMA on executive functioning among this sample, suggesting that young adults with higher levels of SMA tend to have poorer executive functioning, including a series of cognitive processes. Individuals with SMA tend to be unable to control their use of social media successfully, and this symptom is highly related to inhibitory control, which is the process of central executive functioning.58 As a previous experimental study found smartphone addicts had higher mind-wandering frequency and further influenced executive control functioning,59 our findings also supported that SMA was associated with poorer executive functioning. The results of SEM also showed that there were indirect effects from SMA on executive functioning. More specifically, there were two pathways underlying the negative association: one pathway involved emotional disturbance alone, and the other involved both emotional disturbance and sleep quality.

The emotional disturbance was positively related to SMA, as results in previous studies have shown.60,61 This present study further showed individuals with higher SMA reported a higher level of emotional disturbance, which in turn predicted poorer executive functioning. According to previous studies, SMA could contribute to not only more symptoms of emotional disturbance but also emotional regulation deficit,33,62 both of which are associated with the impairment of brain processes and abilities of executive functioning. For example, anxiety and depression have both been found to impair executive functioning and its related brain systems.63,64 A longitudinal study also showed the direct effects of depression and generalized anxiety symptoms on executive functioning impairment after 18 years,34 demonstrating the long-lasting severity of emotional disturbance in the development of executive functioning. Given executive functioning deficits have also been tested as risk factors for anxiety disorder,65 the bidirectional relationship between emotional disturbance and executive functioning should also be tested in further longitudinal studies.

Furthermore, emotional disturbance could also have a negative impact on executive functioning via sleep quality. As one of the basic activities for individuals, sleep plays a vital role in brain and cognitive functions and sleep deprivation would impair executive functioning.42 Poor sleep quality and weak circadian rhythm would make individuals unable to recover from the loss of self-control resources in the long run,40 and further damage or alter the processes and abilities related to executive functioning.66,67 The present study is the first to examine the associations among these negative consequences of SMA (ie, emotional disturbance, poor sleep quality, and poor executive functioning). More attention should be paid to the adverse effects of SMA because there were interactions between such negative consequences from behavioral problems to cognitive impairment. The observations of the significant effect of SMA on executive functioning directly also suggested other potential internal mechanisms exist between SMA and executive functioning, which need to be further investigated. Moreover, there was also a significant and positive relationship between SMA and poor sleep quality, which was consistent with previous studies.68,69 The possible reason may be that a higher level of social media use would reduce melatonin output, and lead to poor sleep quality or sleep problems consequently,70 except for another reason of emotional disturbance found in the present study.

The limitations of the present study should also be mentioned. First, convenience sampling may limit the generalizability of the findings to Chinese young adult populations, and further study should be undertaken among other samples. Second, the cross-sectional study design limits the interpretation of the results for causal/reciprocal relationships among the variables studied, therefore longitudinal or experimental studies should also be conducted. Third, the current measurement tools (eg, executive functioning and sleep quality) were subject to self-reported bias. More experimental designs for executive functioning and technical tools for sleep should also be used for the replicability of the findings in further research.

Despite the limitations, the study demonstrated the negative association between SMA and executive functioning, as well as positive associations with emotional disturbance and sleep quality among Chinese young adults. Not only a direct effect but the indirect effects of SMA by emotional disturbance alone, or by a serial mediation of emotional disturbance and sleep quality on executive functioning was also found in the present study. The present study expands the understanding of the associations between SMA and its negative consequences on emotional processes and cognitive functions. More practical interventions (eg, emotional regulation strategies) should be conducted to alleviate the negative impact of SMA on executive functioning.

Author Contributions

All authors made a significant contribution to the work reported, whether that is in the conception, study design, execution, acquisition of data, analysis and interpretation, or in all these areas; took part in drafting, revising or critically reviewing the article; gave final approval of the version to be published; have agreed on the journal to which the article has been submitted; and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

Funding

The research was supported by grants from the National Science Foundation of China (81771823) and Tianjin Philosophy & Social Science Research (TJJX19-007).

Disclosure

The authors report no conflicts of interest in this work.

References

1. Kuss DJ, Griffiths MD. Social networking sites and addiction: ten lessons learned. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2017;14:311. doi:10.3390/ijerph14030311

2. Wartberg L, Kriston L, Thomasius R. Internet gaming disorder and problematic social media use in a representative sample of German adolescents: prevalence estimates, comorbid depressive symptoms and related psychosocial aspects. Comput Human Behav. 2020;103:31–36. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2019.09.014

3. Zhao L. The impact of social media use types and social media addiction on subjective well-being of college students: a comparative analysis of addicted and non-addicted students. Comput Human Behav Rep. 2021;4:100122. doi:10.1016/j.chbr.2021.100122

4. Statista Research Department. Number of global social network users 2017–2025; 2022. Available from: https://www.statista.com/statistics/278414/number-of-worldwide-social-network-users/. Accessed May 19, 2023.

5. China Internet Network Information Center. The 48th statistical report on China’s internet development; 2021. Available from: http://www.cnnic.cn/hlwfzyj/hlwxzbg/. Accessed May 19, 2023.

6. Verduyn P, Gugushvili N, Kross E. The impact of social network sites on mental health: distinguishing active from passive use. World Psychiatry. 2021;20(1):133. doi:10.1002/wps.20820

7. Valkenburg PM, van Driel II, Beyens I. The associations of active and passive social media use with well-being: a critical scoping review. New Media Soc. 2022;24(2):530–549. doi:10.1177/14614448211065425

8. Ahsan N, Hakim F. Is social media addiction in COVID-19 pandemic affecting students’ academic performance and health? South Asian J Manage. 2021;15(2):254–273.

9. Chen IH, Pakpour AH, Leung H, et al. Comparing generalized and specific problematic smartphone/internet use: longitudinal relationships between smartphone application-based addiction and social media addiction and psychological distress. J Behav Addict. 2020;9(2):410–419. doi:10.1556/2006.2020.00023

10. Lin CY, Namdar P, Griffiths MD, Pakpour AH. Mediated roles of generalized trust and perceived social support in the effects of problematic social media use on mental health: a cross‐sectional study. Health Expect. 2021;24(1):165–173. doi:10.1111/hex.13169

11. Satici B, Kayis AR, Griffiths MD. Exploring the association between social media addiction and relationship satisfaction: psychological distress as a mediator. Int J Ment Health Addict. 2021;20:1–15.

12. Andreassen CS, Pallesen S, Griffiths MD. The relationship between addictive use of social media, narcissism, and self-esteem: findings from a large national survey. Addict Behav. 2017;64:287–293. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2016.03.006

13. Luo T, Qin L, Cheng L, et al. Determination the cut-off point for the Bergen social media addiction (BSMAS): diagnostic contribution of the six criteria of the components model of addiction for social media disorder. J Behav Addict. 2021;10(2):281–290. doi:10.1556/2006.2021.00025

14. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th ed. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing; 2013.

15. Malak MZ, Shuhaiber AH, Al-amer RM, Abuadas MH, Aburoomi RJ. Correlation between psychological factors, academic performance and social media addiction: model-based testing. Behav Inf Technol. 2021;20:1–13.

16. Griffiths MD. A ‘components’ model of addiction within a biopsychosocial framework. J Subst Use. 2005;10(4):191–197. doi:10.1080/14659890500114359

17. Tang CSK, Wu AMS, Yan ECW, et al. Relative risks of Internet-related addictions and mood disturbances among college students: a 7-country/region comparison. Public Health. 2018;165:16–25. doi:10.1016/j.puhe.2018.09.010

18. Nguyen TH, Lin KH, Rahman FF, Ou JP, Wong WK. Study of depression, anxiety, and social media addiction among undergraduate students. J Manage Inform Decis Sci. 2020;23(4):64–76.

19. Liu C, Ma J. Social media addiction and burnout: the mediating roles of envy and social media use anxiety. Curr Psychol. 2020;39(6):1883–1891. doi:10.1007/s12144-018-9998-0

20. Ho TTQ, Nguyen BP, Nguyen TNB, Pham TTH, Thanh T, Mai T. Facebook addiction disorder and sleep quality: loneliness as a mediator. Psychol Educ. 2021;57(7):451–455.

21. Gilbert SJ, Burgess PW. Executive function. Curr Biol. 2008;18(3):R110–R114. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2007.12.014

22. Suchy Y. Executive functioning: overview, assessment, and research issues for non-neuropsychologists. Ann Behav Med. 2009;37(2):106–116. doi:10.1007/s12160-009-9097-4

23. Lewis M. Addiction and the brain: development, not disease. Neuroethics. 2017;10(1):7–18. doi:10.1007/s12152-016-9293-4

24. Noël X, Brevers D, Bechara A. A neurocognitive approach to understanding the neurobiology of addiction. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2013;23(4):632–638. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2013.01.018

25. Bates ME, Pawlak AP, Tonigan JS, Buckman JF. Cognitive impairment influences drinking outcome by altering therapeutic mechanisms of change. Psychol Addict Behav. 2006;20(3):241–253. doi:10.1037/0893-164X.20.3.241

26. Rosselli M, Christopher DM. Executive dysfunctions associated with the use of information technology. Dysexecutive Syndr. 2019;54:177–197.

27. Ioannidis K, Hook R, Goudriaan AE, et al. Cognitive deficits in problematic internet use: meta-analysis of 40 studies. Br J Psychiatry. 2019;215(5):639–646. doi:10.1192/bjp.2019.3

28. Aydın O, Obuća F, Boz C, Ünal-aydın P. Associations between executive functions and problematic social networking sites use. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2020;42(6):634–645. doi:10.1080/13803395.2020.1798358

29. He ZH, Li MD, Ma XY, Liu CJ. Family socioeconomic status and social media addiction in female college students: the mediating role of impulsiveness and inhibitory control. J Genet Psychol. 2021;182(1):60–74. doi:10.1080/00221325.2020.1853027

30. Brailovskaia J, Margraf J. Relationship between depression symptoms, physical activity, and addictive social media use. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. 2020;23(12):818–822. doi:10.1089/cyber.2020.0255

31. Wood M, Center H, Parenteau SC. Social media addiction and psychological adjustment: religiosity and spirituality in the age of social media. Ment Health Relig Cult. 2016;19(9):972–983. doi:10.1080/13674676.2017.1300791

32. Wong HY, Mo HY, Potenza MN, et al. Relationships between severity of internet gaming disorder, severity of problematic social media use, sleep quality and psychological distress. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;17(6):1879. doi:10.3390/ijerph17061879

33. Hormes JM, Kearns B, Timko CA. Craving Facebook? Behavioral addiction to online social networking and its association with emotion regulation deficits. Addiction. 2014;109(12):2079–2088. doi:10.1111/add.12713

34. Zainal NH, Newman MG. Inflammation mediates depression and generalized anxiety symptoms predicting executive function impairment after 18 years. J Affect Disord. 2022;296:465–475. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2021.08.077

35. Eysenck MW, Payne S, Derakshan N. Trait anxiety, visuospatial processing, and working memory. Cogn Emot. 2005;19(8):1214–1228. doi:10.1080/02699930500260245

36. Eysenck MW, Derakshan N, Santos R, Calvo MG. Anxiety and cognitive performance: attentional control theory. Emotion. 2007;7(2):336–353. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.336

37. De Lissnyder E, Derakshan N, De Raedt R, Koster EH. Depressive symptoms and cognitive control in a mixed antisaccade task: specific effects of depressive rumination. Cogn Emot. 2011;25(5):886–897. doi:10.1080/02699931.2010.514711

38. Aldhawyan AF, Alfaraj AA, Elyahia SA, Alshehri SZ, Alghamdi AA. Determinants of subjective poor sleep quality in social media users among freshman college students. Nat Sci Sleep. 2020;12:279–288. doi:10.2147/NSS.S243411

39. Xanidis N, Brignell CM. The association between the use of social network sites, sleep quality and cognitive function during the day. Comput Human Behav. 2016;55:121–126. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2015.09.004

40. Hagger MS. Sleep, self-regulation, self-control and health. Stress Health. 2010;26(3):181. doi:10.1002/smi.1345

41. Ferraro FR, Holfeld B, Frankl S, Frye N, Halvorson N. Texting/iPod dependence, executive function and sleep quality in college students. Comput Human Behav. 2015;49:44–49. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2015.02.043

42. Wang F, Shao Y, Qi J, et al. Effects of total sleep deprivation on executive function in young men. Chin Ment Health J. 2010;24(7):541–545.

43. Hussain Z, Griffiths MD. The associations between problematic social networking site use and sleep quality, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, depression, anxiety and stress. Int J Ment Health Addict. 2021;19(3):686–700. doi:10.1007/s11469-019-00175-1

44. Yang J, Fu X, Liao X, Li Y. Association of problematic smartphone use with poor sleep quality, depression, and anxiety: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychiatry Res. 2020;284:112686. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2019.112686

45. Doğan İ, Doğan N. The prevalence of depression, anxiety, stress and its association with sleep quality among medical students. Ankara Med J. 2019;19(3):550–558.

46. Zhai K, Gao X, Wang G. The role of sleep quality in the psychological well-being of final year undergraduate students in China. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2018;15(12):2881. doi:10.3390/ijerph15122881

47. Andreassen CS, Billieux J, Griffiths MD, et al. The relationship between addictive use of social media and video games and symptoms of psychiatric disorders: a large-scale cross-sectional Study. Psychol Addict Behav. 2016;30(2):252–262. doi:10.1037/adb0000160

48. Andreassen CS, Torsheim T, Brunborg GS, Pallesen S. Development of a Facebook addiction scale. Psychol Rep. 2012;110(2):501–517. doi:10.2466/02.09.18.PR0.110.2.501-517

49. Lovibond PF, Lovibond SH. The structure of negative emotional states: comparison of the depression anxiety stress scales (DASS) with the beck depression and anxiety inventories. Behav Res Ther. 1995;33(3):35–43. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(94)00075-U

50. Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF, Monk TH, Berman SR, Kupfer DJ. The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res. 1989;28(2):193–213. doi:10.1016/0165-1781(89)90047-4

51. Buchanan T, Heffernan TM, Parrott AC, Ling J, Rodgers J, Scholey AB. A short self-report measure of problems with executive function suitable for administration via the internet. Behav Res Methods. 2010;42(3):709–714. doi:10.3758/BRM.42.3.709

52. Sterba SK, Rights JD. Accounting for parcel-allocation variability in practice: combining sources of uncertainty and choosing the number of allocations. Multivariate Behav Res. 2016;51(2–3):296–313. doi:10.1080/00273171.2016.1144502

53. Kline RB. Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling. 3rd ed. Guilford Press; 2011.

54. Hayes A. Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis. J Educ Measure. 2013;51(3):335–337. doi:10.1111/jedm.12050

55. Preacher KJ, Hayes AF. SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput. 2004;36(4):717–731. doi:10.3758/BF03206553

56. Dong G, Lin X, Potenza MN. Decreased functional connectivity in an executive control network is related to impaired executive function in internet gaming disorder. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2015;57:76–85. doi:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2014.10.012

57. Zhou Z, Zhou H, Zhu H. Working memory, executive function and impulsivity in Internet-addictive disorders: a comparison with pathological gambling. Acta Neuropsychiatrica. 2016;28(2):92–100. doi:10.1017/neu.2015.54

58. Wang L, Tian M, Zheng Y, Li Q, Liu X. Reduced loss aversion and inhibitory control in adolescents with internet gaming disorder. Psychol Addict Behav. 2020;34(3):484–496. doi:10.1037/adb0000549

59. Lin Y, Liu Q, Deng H, Li Y, Ding K. Executive control function of smartphone addicts: the role of mind-wandering. Psychological Dev Educ. 2018;34(3):273–283.

60. Abi-Jaoude E, Naylor KT, Pignatiello A. Smartphones, social media use and youth mental health. Can Med Assoc J. 2020;192(6):e136–e141. doi:10.1503/cmaj.190434

61. Henzel V, Håkansson A. Hooked on virtual social life: problematic social media use and associations with mental distress and addictive disorders. PLoS One. 2021;16(4):e0248406. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0248406

62. Hou Y, Xiong D, Jiang T, Song L, Wang Q. Social media addiction: its impact, mediation, and intervention. Cyberpsychology. 2019;13(1):4. doi:10.5817/cp2019-1-4

63. Billingsley‐Marshall RL, Basso MR, Lund BC, et al. Executive function in eating disorders: the role of state anxiety. Int J Eat Disord. 2013;46(4):316–321. doi:10.1002/eat.22086

64. Wang L, LaBar KS, Smoski M, et al. Prefrontal mechanisms for executive control over emotional distraction are altered in major depression. Psychiatry Res Neuroimag. 2008;163(2):143–155. doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2007.10.004

65. Zainal NH, Newman MG. Executive function and other cognitive deficits are distal risk factors of generalized anxiety disorder 9 years later. Psychol Med. 2018;48(12):2045–2053. doi:10.1017/S0033291717003579

66. McKinnon AC, Lagopoulos J, Terpening Z, et al. Sleep disturbance in mild cognitive impairment is associated with alterations in the brain’s default mode network. Behav Neurosci. 2016;130(3):305. doi:10.1037/bne0000137

67. Pessoa L. How do emotion and motivation direct executive control? Trends Cogn Sci. 2009;13(4):160–166. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.006

68. Masoed ES, Omar R, Magd A, Elashry R. Social media addiction among adolescents: its relationship to sleep quality and life satisfaction. Int J Res Paediatr Nurs. 2021;3(1):69–78.

69. Sümen A, Evgin D. Social media addiction in high school students: a cross-sectional study examining its relationship with sleep quality and psychological problems. Child Indic Res. 2021;14(6):2265–2283. doi:10.1007/s12187-021-09838-9

70. Alonzo R, Hussain J, Stranges S, Anderson KK. Interplay between social media use, sleep quality, and mental health in youth: a systematic review. Sleep Med Rev. 2021;56:101414. doi:10.1016/j.smrv.2020.101414

Creative Commons License © 2023 The Author(s). This work is published and licensed by Dove Medical Press Limited. The full terms of this license are available at https://www.dovepress.com/terms.php and incorporate the Creative Commons Attribution - Non Commercial (unported, v3.0) License. By accessing the work you hereby accept the Terms. Non-commercial uses of the work are permitted without any further permission from Dove Medical Press Limited, provided the work is properly attributed. For permission for commercial use of this work, please see paragraphs 4.2 and 5 of our Terms.