Back to Journals » Psychology Research and Behavior Management » Volume 13

Burnout as a State: Random-Intercept Cross-Lagged Relationship Between Exhaustion and Disengagement in a 10-Day Study [Response to Letter]

Authors Basinska BA , Gruszczynska E 

Received 12 May 2020

Accepted for publication 12 May 2020

Published 2 June 2020 Volume 2020:13 Pages 491—493

DOI https://doi.org/10.2147/PRBM.S262432



Beata A Basinska,1 Ewa Gruszczynska2

1Faculty of Management and Economics, Gdansk University of Technology, Gdansk, Poland; 2Faculty of Psychology, University of Social Sciences and Humanities, Warsaw, Poland

Correspondence: Beata A Basinska
Faculty of Management and Economics, Gdansk University of Technology, Narutowicza 11/12, Gdansk 80-233, Poland
Tel +48 58 647 1899
Fax +48 58 347 1861
Email [email protected]

We are writing in response to the Letter to the Editor by Block, Bair, and Carillo1 regarding our study,2 which took a multilevel approach to examine cross-lagged and co-existing relationships between exhaustion and disengagement, measured daily for
ten consecutive working days using the Oldenburg Burnout Inventory (OLBI).3,4 We assessed the job burnout symptoms using only eight items, all of them worded negatively.1

 

This is in response to the Letter to the Editor

 

View the original paper by Basinska and Gruszczynska

Dear Editor,

We are writing in response to the Letter to the Editor by Block, Bair, and Carillo1 regarding our study,2 which took a multilevel approach to examine cross-lagged and co-existing relationships between exhaustion and disengagement, measured daily for ten consecutive working days using the Oldenburg Burnout Inventory (OLBI).3,4 We assessed the job burnout symptoms using only eight items, all of them worded negatively.1

Following Block, Bair, and Carillo’s1 request, we are happy to provide supplementary cross-sectional results from the first day of our study. The mean and standard deviation was 2.34 ± 0.99 for exhaustion, 2.10 ± 0.80 for disengagement, and the t-paired test was statistically significant (t (234) = 5.47, p < 0.001). In addition, a higher level of exhaustion was associated with a higher level of disengagement (Pearson’s r = 0.72, p <.001).

Furthermore, we have datasets from other studies58 on burnout, in which the first author was involved, that use OLBI as a measurement tool and we are presenting similar analyses based on them as well. The summary is provided in Table 1.

Table 1 The Difference Between Levels of Exhaustion and Disengagement and the Correlation Between Both Components of Job Burnout in a Cross-Sectional Measurement

In the study among civil servants,5 we examined the shape of the relationship between positivity (i.e., the proportion of positive to negative job-related emotions) and job burnout using a cross-sectional and longitudinal approach. Job burnout was measured twice, at a four-month interval. We observed that, at each measurement point, the mean value for exhaustion was significantly higher than for disengagement. This is consistent with results from an academic anesthesia department reported by Block, Bair, and Carillo.9 We also examined the relationship between positivity and job burnout among a mixed group of police officers and firefighters.6 Contrary to the aforementioned studies, the results of this study indicated that the mean value for disengagement was higher than for exhaustion. However, it is worth noting that the gender distribution of each group differed greatly. While the majority of civil servants were women (75%), the vast majority of uniformed officers were men (94%). Also, the characteristics of these professions differ substantially. In another study, Basinska, Wiciak, and Dåderman7 examined the relationship between fatigue, emotion, and job burnout among a group of police officers (15% women) where the results also showed that the level of disengagement was higher than the level of exhaustion. Furthermore, Basinska and Dåderman8 examined how work values were associated with job burnout and work engagement among police officers (21% women) and found no significant difference between levels of exhaustion and disengagement. Therefore, the findings of cross-sectional comparisons may be influenced by profession as well as gender, or else other contextual and sample-specific factors, and should not be interpreted as universal. Nonetheless, the result replicated in all these studies was a moderate to strong positive correlation between exhaustion and disengagement (range of Pearson’s correlation coefficient from 0.55 to 0.82).

Additionally, since Baka and Basinska4 developed stanine norms for exhaustion and disengagement for the Polish version of OLBI, it is possible to evaluate the level of burnout in a more objective manner for the cited studies. According to these norms, scores above 2.75 for exhaustion and above 2.72 for disengagement indicate a high level of these components. Consequently, the uniformed officers reported, on average, a moderate level of exhaustion and disengagement. However, it should be emphasized that a high level of job burnout (above the cut-off point) is not equivalent to a clinical stage of burnout. In fact, prior studies suggest that exhaustion, in particular, may be strongly associated with a clinical diagnosis of burnout.1012 Thus, possible differences in the stage of burnout development between samples is another variable that should be taken into account when interpreting the results.

We would like to thank Block, Bair, and Carillo1 for their letter as it allowed us to situate our study in a broader context and present additional results for comparison. We also believe that even relatively straightforward analysis may inspire future studies on burnout structure and dynamics, especially if they result in research questions addressed by more advanced models that decompose burnout variance into between-person differences and within-person fluctuations.

Funding

This paper was supported by a grant from the National Science Centre, Poland [Research project no. UMO-2015/17/B/HS6/04178].

Disclosure

The authors declare that there is no conflict of interest regarding this communication.

References

1. Block RI, Bair HL, Carillo JF. Response to burnout as a state: random-intercept cross-lagged relationship 1 between exhaustion and disengagement in a 10-day study [letter]. Psychol Res Behav Manag. 2020;13:395–396. doi:10.2147/PRBM.S258125

2. Basinska BA, Gruszczynska E. Burnout as a state: random-intercept cross-lagged relationship between exhaustion and disengagement in a 10-day study. Psychol Res Behav Manag. 2020;13:267–278. doi:10.2147/PRBM.S244397

3. Demerouti E, Mostert K, Bakker AB. Burnout and work engagement: a thorough investigation of the independency of both constructs. J Occup Health Psychol. 2010;15(3):209–222. doi:10.1037/a0019408

4. Baka L, Basinska BA. Psychometryczne wlasciwosci polskiej wersji Oldenburskiego Kwestionariusza Wypalenia Zawodowego (OLBI) [Psychometric properties of the polish version of the Oldenburg Burnout Inventory (OLBI)]. Med Pr. 2016;67(1):29–42. doi:10.13075/mp.5893.00353

5. Basinska B, Gruszczynska E. Job-related emotions and job burnout among civil servants: examining the shape of the relationship in cross-sectional and longitudinal models. Med Pr. 2019;70(2):201–211. doi:10.13075/mp.5893.00750

6. Basinska BA, Gruszczynska E. Positivity and job burnout in emergency personnel: examining linear and curvilinear relationship. Pol Psychol Bull. 2017;48(2):212–219. doi:10.1515/ppb-2017-0024

7. Basinska BA, Wiciak I, Dåderman AM. Fatigue and burnout in police officers: the mediating role of emotions. Policing. 2014;37(3):665–680. doi:10.1108/PIJPSM-10-2013-0105

8. Basinska BA, Dåderman AM. Work values of police officers and their relationship with job burnout and work engagement. Front Psychol. 2019;10:442. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00442

9. Block RI, Bair HL, Carillo JF. Is exhaustion more sensitive than disengagement to burnout in academic anesthesia? A study using the oldenburg burnout inventory. Psychol Rep. 2019;003329411985656. doi:10.1177/0033294119856560

10. Oosterholt BG, Maes JH, Van der Linden D, Verbraak MJ, Kompier MA. Burnout and cortisol: evidence for a lower cortisol awakening response in both clinical and non-clinical burnout. J Psychosom Res. 2015;78(5):445–451. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychores.2014.11.003

11. Roelofs J, Verbraak M, Keijsers GP, De Bruin MB, Schmidt AJ. Psychometric properties of a dutch version of the maslach burnout inventory general survey (MBI‐DV) in individuals with and without clinical burnout. Stress Health. 2005;21(1):17–25. doi:10.1002/smi.1032

12. Schaufeli WB, Bakker AB, Hoogduin K, Schaap C, Kladler A. On the clinical validity of the maslach burnout inventory and the burnout measure. Psychol Health. 2001;16(5):565–582. doi:10.1080/08870440108405527

Creative Commons License © 2020 The Author(s). This work is published and licensed by Dove Medical Press Limited. The full terms of this license are available at https://www.dovepress.com/terms.php and incorporate the Creative Commons Attribution - Non Commercial (unported, v3.0) License. By accessing the work you hereby accept the Terms. Non-commercial uses of the work are permitted without any further permission from Dove Medical Press Limited, provided the work is properly attributed. For permission for commercial use of this work, please see paragraphs 4.2 and 5 of our Terms.