Back to Journals » International Journal of Women's Health » Volume 11

An educational intervention based on the extended parallel process model to improve attitude, behavioral intention, and early breast cancer diagnosis: a randomized trial

Authors Termeh Zonouzy V, Niknami S, Ghofranipour F , Montazeri A 

Received 4 August 2018

Accepted for publication 25 November 2018

Published 18 December 2018 Volume 2019:11 Pages 1—10

DOI https://doi.org/10.2147/IJWH.S182146

Checked for plagiarism Yes

Review by Single anonymous peer review

Peer reviewer comments 2

Editor who approved publication: Professor Elie Al-Chaer



Vahideh Termeh Zonouzy,1 Shamsaddin Niknami,1 Fazlollah Ghofranipour,1 Ali Montazeri2

1Department of Health Education and Promotion, Faculty of Medical Sciences, Tarbiat Modares University, Tehran, Iran; 2Population Health Research Group, Health Metrics Research Center, Iranian Institute for Health Sciences Research, ACECR, Tehran, Iran

Background: Breast cancer is an important public health problem worldwide. This study aimed to assess the effectiveness of an educational intervention based on fear appeals using the extended parallel process model (EPPM) to improve attitudes, intention, and early breast cancer diagnosis in Iranian women.
Methods: This was a cluster-randomized trial conducted in Tehran, Iran. A sample of women 40 years old and above was recruited and assigned either to intervention group or to control group. The intervention group received two pamphlets containing fear appeals specially designed for this study using the EPPM. The control group received nothing. The primary outcomes were changes in attitude, behavioral intention, and early breast cancer diagnosis. Participants responded to a questionnaire before intervention and at 3 months follow-up assessments. Independent t-tests and paired t-tests were used for comparison.
Result: In all, 600 women were entered into the study (277 in the intervention group and 323 in the control group). Of these, 162 were lost to follow-up assessment. Thus, the analysis was restricted to those for whom both baseline and follow-up information was available (n=438). The mean age of participants was 53.2 (SD=9.45) years, and the mean year education of participants was 9.4 (SD=4.12) years. There were no significant differences between two groups regarding demographic variables. However, comparing outcome variables (attitude, intention, and early breast cancer diagnosis) at baseline and follow-up assessments, we found that the intervention group showed significant improvements in attitude and intention (P=0.01 and P=0.001, respectively), but no significant improvement was observed for early breast cancer diagnosis (P=0.78). The control group did not show any changes.
Conclusion: The findings showed that fear appeals could not influence early breast cancer diagnosis among women. However, significant changes were observed for attitude and behavioral intention.

Keywords: EPPM, breast cancer screening, women’s health, Iran

Introduction

Breast cancer is the most common cancer among women both in developed and developing countries.16 Each year 1.5 million women are diagnosed with breast cancer and ~15% die from the disease globally.7 Thus, early detection of breast cancer is of paramount importance. Several strategies exist for early detection of breast cancer including raising awareness among women especially those who are at higher risk of the disease.1,812 As such, providing necessary information to encourage women for early breast cancer diagnosis is a routine practice by health professionals worldwide.6,1317 It is argued that awareness might persuade women to take appropriate actions toward breast cancer care.1012,1820 However, studies showed that the effectiveness of health education interventions and health messages highly depends on whether they are theory-driven or not.2124 Indeed, using persuasive messages applying fear appeals in attempt to influence women’s behaviors toward early breast cancer diagnosis is common in health education and health promotion programs.11,2535 The most recent development in this area backs to year 1992 where the extended parallel process model (EPPM) was proposed. The model argues that a fear appeal might work if certain conditions exist that include susceptibility and severity, self-efficacy, and response efficacy.11,33,3641

Susceptibility refers to one’s perception to vulnerability to the risk (eg, all women are prone to breast cancer). Severity refers to seriousness of the consequences if the recommended suggestions are not followed (eg, breast cancer is a fatal and serious disease and affects your family life and work). Self-efficacy refers to a person’s belief about his/her ability to follow the recommended suggestions successfully (eg, women can learn about early breast cancer diagnosis and manage their time to accomplish annual clinical breast examination). Response efficacy refers to beliefs about the effectiveness of the recommended suggestions to avert the threat (eg, annual clinical breast examination helps in early detection of breast cancer and thus a successful treatment).32,34,35,38,39,42,43 The schematic picture of the EPPM is illustrated in Figure 1.

Figure 1 A schematic view of the extended parallel process model.

The current study was an attempt to use the EPPM to encourage Iranian women to take necessary actions toward early detection of breast cancer. Breast cancer in Iran is increasing rapidly and unfortunately kills many women due to late diagnosis and poor treatment.1,2,5,8,9,12,13,16,4448 It was hoped that the study results could help to improve women’s health in general and breast care in particular.

Methods

Trial design

This was a cluster-randomized trial with two parallel groups (intervention and control). This was chosen to avoid contamination between intervention and control groups.4951 Data were collected at baseline and 3 months after intervention. The study started at the beginning of October 2017. After about 3 months, follow-up assessments started at December 29, 2017.

Study setting

The study was conducted in Tehran, Iran. Tehran, a metropolitan city, is the capital of Iran and has a multicultural composite.

Participants

Eligible cases for this study were women who were at least 40 years old, could read and write, did not have a history of breast cancer herself or in her first-degree family, and did not have psychiatric disorders. Women were excluded if they did not meet the inclusion criteria.

Intervention

The intervention group received two pamphlets as educational materials, which were designed based on the EPPM. It is believed that written materials are the most commonly used, cost-effective, and time-efficient approach to communicate health messages.11,25,40,52

As explained before, the EPPM constructs are perceived threat (susceptibility and severity) and perceived efficacy (self-efficacy and response efficacy). Pamphlets contained messages designed based on these constructs and described about severity and hardness of the disease, and its painful and difficult outcomes on patients and their family, work, and community. A short clarification about cure modalities including surgery and chemotherapy was explained to make women conscious about the consequences of delay in seeking behaviors for breast cancer symptoms. In susceptibility section, it was explained that breast cancer is rising in Iran and all women are vulnerable in developing breast cancer, and some critical signs of breast cancer were demonstrated. Also it was announced that early diagnosis might proceed to favorable medication. The next part included response efficacy that explained visiting a physician when women see any signs and conducting annual clinical breast cancer examination are useful and effective behaviors in early detection of the disease. The last part presented self-efficacy messages that prescribed women to gain knowledge about breast cancer and arrange their time to do recommended behaviors. Also it was explained that the suggested behaviors are uncomplicated, effortless, and easily done. In assessing the content, a number of health education specialists and 15 women (who were similar to the study samples and were not included in the study) read and examined the pamphlets to make sure that the designed messages were simple, understandable, and frightening. The two pamphlets were organized differently in literature and pictures.

Control

The participants were allocated into control group at the study commence. They did not receive anything. This group received educational materials used in this study (pamphlets) after completion of the study.

Outcomes

Attitude, behavioral intention, and behavior scores were the primary outcomes and evaluated at baseline and 3 months follow-up by a questionnaire.

Measures

A questionnaire was designed specifically for the study in order to measure the study outcomes. The instrument consisted of the following sections:

  1. Demographic characteristics: including items on age, education, employment, and marital status.
  2. Attitude: it consisted of 15 questions on women’s belief and emotions about early breast cancer diagnosis and its importance with a five-point Likert scale response category ranging from completely agree to completely disagree. These were coded from 5 to 1, respectively. Then, overall attitude score was calculated by summing all items scored, giving a score ranging from 15 to 75. The higher score indicates the more positive attitude toward early breast cancer diagnosis.
  3. Behavioral intention: two questions on women’s desire and wishes to attend for annual clinical breast cancer examination and physician visit when they see any sign in their breast. The response categories were “agree, no idea, and do not agree.” These were coded as 2, 1, and 0, respectively. Then, overall behavioral intention score was calculated by summing all items scored, giving a score ranging from 0 to 4. The higher score indicates the more positive behavioral intention toward early breast cancer diagnosis.
  4. Behavior: two statements made the last part of the measure. Women were asked to answer whether they attended for annual clinical breast cancer examination and visited physician when they see an unusual sign in their breast. The response choices were “Yes, I do and No, I do not”, corresponding to 1 and 0 scores, respectively. Then, overall behavior score was calculated by summing all items scored, giving a score ranging from 0 to 2. The higher score indicates better behavior in women toward early breast cancer diagnosis.

To assess validity of the questionnaire, content and face validity were performed. Also reliability of the questionnaire was evaluated by Cronbach’s alpha coefficient.

Sample size

Randomized block stratum was used in this study to achieve enough sample size. Tehran has 22 districts. Each district was considered as a stratum. Then within each stratum a number of blocks (clusters) were selected. Randomization was used to allocate the study groups (intervention or control). The number of clusters were related to population size of each district (stratum). There were more blocks in populated districts. Accordingly, a sample of 300 women in each study group (intervention and control) was estimated to achieve 80% power at 0.05 significance level. There were 29 clusters in intervention group and 35 clusters in control group. Ten women were estimated for each group based on experiments and near blocks added for sampling when the numbers of samples were not enough.5356 Distribution-based clustering was used in this study.57 Table 1 presents the sampling construction.

Table 1 The study sample size and clusters in 22 districts in Tehran, Iran

Recruitment

Study samples were assigned to the two trial arms in equal quantities. The research team presented the allocation before home visits.

Sequence generation

To allocate blocks randomly as intervention or control groups, the Excel software was used. Some directions were applied to get the intended number of samples in the study. Tehran has 22 districts, which vary in socioeconomic and cultural status, so the study was conducted in all these parts. According to every district’s partake of all samples, some blocks were located and assigned as intervention or control randomly.

Implementation

Blocks were assigned as intervention or control by a statistician who did not take part in the study. Some skilled female interviewers learned about the study and were trained on how to collect data with the questionnaire. At first, they enacted home visits in selected parts (blocks), described the study intention, and made sure about inclusion criteria. Participants completed the written consent, and their phone number was taken for follow-up assessments. Participants were assured for confidentiality in the study. Research team monitored all study stages carefully to ascertain for correct implementation.

Blinding

No blinding was used in this study.

Statistical analysis

First, we calculated attitude, intention, and behavior scores for each respondent. As such, the score for attitude ranged from 15 to 75, for intention it ranged from 0 to 4, and for behavior it ranged from 0 to 2. In all instances, the higher scores indicated better conditions. Then, data from baseline and follow-up assessments were analyzed using mean and standard deviations, percentages, chi-square, t-test, and paired t-test. Finally, logistic regression analysis was performed using adjusted difference-in-difference analyses. In doing so, the follow-up scores were treated as dependent variables while baseline level of the outcomes and other individual characteristics were considered as independent variables.

Ethics

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and the ethics committee of Tarbiat Modares University approved the study. All participants were asked to sign an informed consent form, and they were assured that all information provided would be kept confidential.

Results

In all, 600 women were entered into the study (277 participants in intervention and 323 participants in control group). However, at follow-up, a number of participants were missed due to change in address (n=100) and dislike (n=62), giving 438 participants for whom both baseline and follow-up data were available for analysis (217 in intervention group and 221 in control group). The study flowchart is shown in Figure 2. The mean age of participants was 53.2 (SD=9.45) years. There were no significant differences between the study groups with regard to age, education, employment, and marital status. The findings are shown in Table 2.

Figure 2 The study flowchart.

Table 2 Demographic information of the study groups at baseline and follow-up assessments
Note: aDerived from t-test and chi-squared test as appropriate.

At baseline, there were no significant differences between the intervention and control groups in outcome measures (Table 3). However, at follow-up assessment, there were significant differences between the two study groups, indicating that the intervention group showed significant improvements in attitudes (P=0.03) and behavioral intention (P=0.01) but not for behavior (Table 4).

Table 3 Comparison of baseline assessment between the study groups (n=600)
Notes: aDerived from chi-squared test. bDerived from t-test.

Table 4 Comparison of follow-up assessment between the study groups (n=438)
Notes: aDerived from chi-squared test. bDerived from t-test.

The paired t-test showed significant changes in attitude and behavioral intention at baseline and follow-up assessments in intervention group (P=0.01 and P=0.001, respectively), while we did not observe such improvements in control group (Table 5).

Table 5 Comparison of the study outcomes at baseline and follow-up within intervention and control groups
Note: aDerived from paired t-test.

Finally, as explained in the Methods section, we performed difference-in-difference analysis to assess the association between the follow-up outcomes while controlling for baseline level of the outcomes and other individual characteristics. The results are presented in Table 6. As shown, baseline levels of the outcomes were significantly associated with the follow-up outcomes. However, for attitude in addition to baseline score, the OR for lower level of outcome in the control group was higher than that in the intervention group (OR=1.30, 95% CI=1.13–161, P=0.04). Again, this ratio for behavioral intention in the control group was higher than that in the intervention group (OR=1.77, 95% CI=1.02–3.05, P=0.03). For behavior, only baseline score was significant and other variables did not show any differences either among the study groups or for other characteristics studied.

Table 6 The OR for lower levels of outcomes at follow-up adjusted for baseline level of the outcomes and women’s characteristics
Abbreviation: ref., reference.

Discussion

The findings from this study indicated that encouraging women for early breast cancer diagnosis using fear appeal did not work. However, the findings showed that fear appeal might be useful message strategy for changing attitude and behavior intention. Studies showed that high threat and high efficacy messages help to influence attitudes and intentions toward recommended behaviors.28,29,41,5964 It is argued that high threat messages could make people feel vulnerable to undergo treatment if they do not follow the recommended messages, and thus, such messages might influence individuals’ attitudes and intentions.59,61

According to the EPPM, when exposing to threatening messages if an individual believes that he/she could manage the threat and have ability to overcome it successfully, then danger control process starts and the fear strategy might work. Thus, it is believed that when using fear appeal, we should try to move individuals to danger control process. Danger control is a cognitive process and leads to changing person’s attitudes, behavioral intention, and behaviors.32,33,41,5864 A meta-analysis of 88 attitude–behavior studies reported that attitudes predict the upcoming behavior considerably and substantially.65 Intention is an important key on individual’s readiness for action. Several studies indicated that intention forecasts behaviors although intention–behavior gap should not be neglected.66,67

The current study showed that there was no significant improvement in women’s behavior toward early breast cancer diagnosis. Two reasons may be put forward to explain the findings. First, there are several factors that influence the effectiveness of fear appeals, including individual characters, norms, strength of fear, perceived threat, and perceived response efficacy.23,68 Therefore, if we could not see significant changes in women’s behavior as a result of the study intervention, this should be interpreted in the light of these factors. For instance, the amount of fear was not enough or women felt that they could not attend the annual clinical breast examination for any reason. Second, our observation might be due to the nature of the recommended behavior or the fact that improvement in one’s behavior could not be captured in a short-term basis. Perhaps the short follow-up assessment (women were followed only for 3 months) did not allow us to really see whether fear appeal could influence women’s behavior or not. The theoretical models of behavior change usually expect a longer time for modifying a problematic behavior or acquiring a positive and healthy behavior. For instance, the transtheoretical model argues that health behavior change involves different long-term processes and thus one should not expect to see a behavior change in a short period of time. The model also posits that not only a behavior change by itself is an important step toward adapting a healthy behavior but also the maintenance of such behaviors is crucial.68

Above all as indicated in the regression analysis (Table 6), baseline level of the outcomes (attitude, behavioral intention, and behavior scores) showed significant association with the same outcome levels at follow-up. Such observation might indicate that those who usually do not have positive attitude toward early breast cancer diagnosis or do not have any intentions to act toward early breast cancer diagnosis regardless of exposure to fear appeals or other strategies might not follow recommended messages or benefit from interventions alone unless we raise awareness among such individuals.

It is known that there are several barriers, which inhibit the early breast cancer diagnosis, such as poverty, lack of health insurance, and aging. Also not having enough knowledge about screening methods, being busy, negligence, forgetfulness, and not feeling necessity for early breast cancer diagnosis are some other reasons that have been acknowledged in different studies.3,12

The women who participated in this study were not well educated. Studies have shown that low-educated women usually do not have enough information about breast cancer, its risk factors, and are most prone to not doing screening behaviors.12,14,17 However, although education is a main factor in determining women’s socioeconomic status, there is a paradox between educational level and risk of breast cancer. Women in higher socioeconomic status usually are at higher risk of breast cancer, while women from lower socioeconomic position are at lower risk of breast cancer but they usually do not use preventive services.12,17 Studies from Iran found that Iranian women have relatively inadequate knowledge about breast cancer and its preventive measures. As such, it has been emphasized that Iranian women need more education and appropriate information about breast cancer and its early detection.2,9,13,47

The participants of this study were selected from women aged 40 years and above living in Tehran. This might limit the external validity of the study although there is evidence that a representative sample from Tehran could be a representative of Iranian people.69 We did not use the risk behavior diagnosis to assess whether women were at danger control or fear control processes at different phases. Short follow-up time is also another limitation. Further studies are needed to investigate how fear appeal strategy might work in the long term in changing early breast cancer diagnosis among women. One another limitation for the current study is the fact that we did not collect information concerning women’s perception of benefits and intangible costs of breast screening (eg, false positive or overdiagnosis). This perception could be a confounding variable in the analysis. The recent literature suggests that in addition to raising awareness among women about early detection and breast cancer screening programs, persuasive communications should focus on informed decision-making in light of potential benefits and harms perceived by women.70,71 Finally, it is important to report that there was a substantial loss to follow-up. This means that the study did not achieve its designed power and this should be considered as a limitation.

Conclusion

The results showed that fear appeals could influence women’s attitude and behavioral intention but not early breast cancer diagnosis. Perhaps longitudinal studies are needed to be able to capture behavior changes when studying early cancer diagnosis.

Data sharing statement

The data presented in this paper are available from the corresponding authors on reasonable request.

Acknowledgments

This manuscript originated from a PhD thesis by VTZ at Department of Health Education and Promotion, Faculty of Medical Sciences, Tarbiat Modares University, Tehran, Iran. We acknowledge the help received from Mrs Koosha and her colleagues in Motamed Breast Cancer Institute, Breast Cancer Research Institute (IBCRC). The research team also is grateful to Iranian Students Polling Agency (ISPA) for their help.

Disclosure

The authors report no conflicts of interest in this work.


References

1.

Haji-Mahmoodi M, Montazeri A, Jarvandi S, Ebrahimi M, Haghighat S, Harirchi I. Breast self-examination: knowledge, attitudes, and practices among female health care workers in Tehran, Iran. Breast J. 2002;8(4):222–225.

2.

Mousavi SM, Montazeri A, Mohagheghi MA, et al. Breast cancer in Iran: an epidemiological review. Breast J. 2007;13(4):383–391.

3.

Memon ZA, Shaikh AN, Rizwan S, Sardar MB. Reasons for patient’s delay in diagnosis of breast carcinoma in Pakistan. Asian Pac J Cancer Prev. 2013;14(12):7409–7414.

4.

Biglu MH. Breast cancer in Iran: the trend of Iranian researchers’ studies in MEDLINE database. Basic Clin Cancer Res. 2014;6:22–32.

5.

Ebrahimi M, Vahdaninia M, Montazeri A. Risk factors for breast cancer in Iran: a case-control study. Breast Cancer Res. 2002;4(5):R10.

6.

Kruk J. Lifestyle components and primary breast cancer prevention. Asian Pac J Cancer Prev. 2014;15(24):10543–10555.

7.

WHO. Breast cancer prevention. Available from: http://www.who.int/cancer/prevention/diagnosis-screening/breast-cancer/en/. Accessed January 1, 2018.

8.

Montazeri A, Ebrahimi M, Mehrdad N, Ansari M, Sajadian A. Delayed presentation in breast cancer: a study in Iranian women. BMC Womens Health. 2003;3(1):4.

9.

Montazeri A, Harirchi I, Vahdani M, et al. Anxiety and depression in Iranian breast cancer patients before and after diagnosis. Eur J Cancer Care. 2000;9(3):151–157.

10.

Boulos DN, Ghali RR. Awareness of breast cancer among female students at Ain Shams University, Egypt. Glob J Health Sci. 2013;6(1):154–161.

11.

Davis SM, Martinelli D, Braxton B, Kutrovac K, Crocco T. The impact of the extended parallel process model on stroke awareness. Stroke. 2009;40(12):3857–3863.

12.

Montazeri A, Vahdaninia M, Harirchi I, et al. Breast cancer in Iran: need for greater women awareness of warning signs and effective screening methods. Asia Pac Fam Med. 2008;7:6.

13.

Mousavi SM, Harirchi I, Ebrahimi M, et al. Screening for breast cancer in Iran: a challenge for health policy makers. Breast J. 2008;14(6):605–606.

14.

Harris R, Yeatts J, Kinsinger L. Breast cancer screening for women ages 50 to 69 years a systematic review of observational evidence. Prev Med. 2011;53(3):108–114.

15.

Farma KK, Jalili Z, Zareban I, Pour MS. Effect of education on preventive behaviors of breast cancer in female teachers of guidance schools of Zahedan city based on health belief model. J Educ Health Promot. 2014;3:77.

16.

Howell A, Anderson AS, Clarke RB, et al. Risk determination and prevention of breast cancer. Breast Cancer Res. 2014;16(5):446.

17.

Ahmadian M, Samah AA. A literature review of factors influencing breast cancer screening in Asian countries. Life Sci J. 2012;9:585–594.

18.

Shinde SD, Kadam SB. Breast cancer awareness among women in Vikhroli: a suburban area of Mumbai, Maharashtra, India. Int J Community Med Public Health. 2017;3:2281–2286.

19.

Evans RE, Morris M, Sekhon M, et al. Increasing awareness of gynaecological cancer symptoms: a GP perspective. Br J Gen Pract. 2014;64(623):e372–e380.

20.

Lostao L, Joiner TE, Pettit JW, Chorot P, Sandín B. Health beliefs and illness attitudes as predictors of breast cancer screening attendance. Eur J Public Health. 2001;11(3):274–279.

21.

Glanz K, Rimer BK, Viswanath K. Health Behavior and Health Education: Theory, Research, and Practice. 4th ed. New York: John Wiley; 2008.

22.

Ruiter RA, Kessels LT, Peters GJ, Kok G. Sixty years of fear appeal research: current state of the evidence. Int J Psychol. 2014;49(2):63–70.

23.

Peters GJ, Ruiter RA, Kok G. Threatening communication: a qualitative study of fear appeal effectiveness beliefs among intervention developers, policymakers, politicians, scientists, and advertising professionals. Int J Psychol. 2014;49(2):71–79.

24.

Murray-Johnson L, Witte K, Boulay M, Figueroa ME, Storey D, Tweedie I. Using health education theories to explain behavior change: a cross-country analysis. 2000–2001. Int Q Community Health Educ. 2005;25(1–2):185–207.

25.

Noar SM, Hall MG, Francis DB, Ribisl KM, Pepper JK, Brewer NT. Pictorial cigarette pack warnings: a meta-analysis of experimental studies. Tob Control. 2016;25(3):341–354.

26.

Nandakumar N, Sivakumaran B, Kalro A, Sharma P. Threat, efficacy and message framing in consumer healthcare. Market Intell Plann. 2017;35(4):442–457.

27.

Hall IJ, Johnson-Turbes A. Use of the persuasive health message framework in the development of a community-based mammography promotion campaign. Cancer Causes Control. 2015;26(5):775–784.

28.

Shi JJ, Smith SW. The effects of fear appeal message repetition on perceived threat, perceived efficacy, and behavioral intention in the extended parallel process model. Health Commun. 2016;31(3):275–286.

29.

Paek HJ, Oh SH, Hove T. How fear-arousing news messages affect risk perceptions and intention to talk about risk. Health Commun. 2016;31(9):1051–1062.

30.

Dillard JP, Anderson JW. The role of fear in persuasion. Psychol Market. 2004;21(11):909–926.

31.

Nan X, Madden K, Richards A, Holt C, Wang MQ, Tracy K. Message framing, perceived susceptibility, and intentions to vaccinate children against HPV among African American parents. Health Commun. 2016;31(7):798–805.

32.

Lewis I, Watson B, White KM. Extending the explanatory utility of the EPPM beyond fear-based persuasion. Health Commun. 2013;28(1):84–98.

33.

Stephenson MT, Witte K, Fear WK. Fear, threat, and perceptions of efficacy from frightening skin cancer messages. Public Health Rev. 1998;26(2):147–174.

34.

Batchelder A, Matusitz J. “Let’s Move” campaign: applying the extended parallel process model. Soc Work Public Health. 2014;29(5):462–472.

35.

Thrasher JF, Swayampakala K, Borland R, et al. Influences of self-efficacy, response efficacy, and reactance on responses to cigarette health warnings: a longitudinal study of adult smokers in Australia and Canada. Health Commun. 2016;31(12):1517–1526.

36.

Hatchell AC, Bassett-Gunter RL, Clarke M, Kimura S, Latimer-Cheung AE. Messages for men: the efficacy of EPPM-based messages targeting men’s physical activity. Health Psychol. 2013;32(1):24–32.

37.

Mckay DL, Berkowitz JM, Blumberg JB, Goldberg JP. Communicating cardiovascular disease risk due to elevated homocysteine levels: using the EPPM to develop print materials. Health Educ Behav. 2004;31(3):355–371.

38.

So J. A further extension of the Extended Parallel Process Model (E-EPPM): implications of cognitive appraisal theory of emotion and dispositional coping style. Health Commun. 2013;28(1):72–83.

39.

Lavoie NR, Quick BL. What is the truth? An application of the extended parallel process model to televised truth® ads. Health Commun. 2013;28(1):53–62.

40.

Fehrenbach KS. Designing Messages to Reduce Meat Consumption: A Test of the Extended Parallel Process Model [PhD dissertation]. AZ, USA: Arizona State University; 2015.

41.

Latour MS, Snipes RL, Bliss SJ. Don’t be afraid to use fear appeals: an experimental study. J Advert Res. 1996;36:59–68.

42.

Krieger JL, Sarge MA. A serial mediation model of message framing on intentions to receive the human papillomavirus (HPV) vaccine: revisiting the role of threat and efficacy perceptions. Health Commun. 2013;28(1):5–19.

43.

Napper LE, Harris PR, Klein WM. Combining self-affirmation with the extended parallel process model: the consequences for motivation to eat more fruit and vegetables. Health Commun. 2014;29(6):610–618.

44.

Majidi A, Salimzadeh H, Beiki O, et al. Cancer research priorities and gaps in Iran: the influence of cancer burden on cancer research outputs between 1997 and 2014. Public Health. 2017;144:42–47.

45.

Enayatrad M, Amoori N, Salehiniya H. Epidemiology and trends in breast cancer mortality in Iran. Iran J Public Health. 2015;44(3):430–431.

46.

Khazaee-Pool M, Montazeri A, Majlessi F, Rahimi Foroushani A, Nedjat S, Shojaeizadeh D. Breast cancer-preventive behaviors: exploring Iranian women’s experiences. BMC Womens Health. 2014;14(1):41.

47.

Ataollahi M, Sedighi S, Masoumi SZ. Nutritional and unhealthy behaviors in women with and without breast cancer. Iran Red Crescent Med J. 2014;16(9):e19684.

48.

Bland JM, Kerry SM. Trials randomised in clusters. BMJ. 1997;315(7108):600.

49.

Campbell MK, Mollison J, Steen N, Grimshaw JM, Eccles M. Analysis of cluster randomized trials in primary care: a practical approach. Fam Pract. 2000;17(2):192–196.

50.

Moberg J, Kramer M. A brief history of the cluster randomised trial design. J R Soc Med. 2015;108(5):192–198.

51.

Cueva K, Cueva M, Dignan M, Landis K. Print material in cancer prevention: an evaluation of three booklets designed with and for Alaska’s community health workers. J Cancer Educ. 2016;31(2):279–284.

52.

van Breukelen GJ, Candel MJ. Calculating sample sizes for cluster randomized trials: we can keep it simple and efficient! J Clin Epidemiol. 2012;65(11):1212–1218.

53.

Eldridge SM, Ashby D, Kerry S. Sample size for cluster randomized trials: effect of coefficient of variation of cluster size and analysis method. Int J Epidemiol. 2006;35(5):1292–1300.

54.

Rutterford C, Copas A, Eldridge S. Methods for sample size determination in cluster randomized trials. Int J Epidemiol. 2015;44(3):1051–1067.

55.

Campbell MK, Thomson S, Ramsay CR, Maclennan GS, Grimshaw JM. Sample size calculator for cluster randomized trials. Comput Biol Med. 2004;34(2):113–125.

56.

Donner A, Birkett N, Buck C. Randomization by cluster. Sample size requirements and analysis. Am J Epidemiol. 1981;114(6):906–914.

57.

Fraley C, Raftery E. How many clusters? Which clustering method? Answers via model-based cluster analysis. Comput J. 1998;41(8):578–588.

58.

Hine DW, Gifford R. Fear appeals, individual differences, and environmental concern. J Environ Educ. 1991;23(1):36–41.

59.

Emery SL, Szczypka G, Abril EP, Kim Y, Vera L. Are you scared yet? Evaluating fear appeal messages in tweets about the tips campaign. J Commun. 2014;64:278–295.

60.

Tannenbaum MB, Hepler J, Zimmerman RS, et al. Appealing to fear: a meta-analysis of fear appeal effectiveness and theories. Psychol Bull. 2015;141(6):1178–1204.

61.

Redmond ML, Dong F, Frazier LM. Does the extended parallel process model fear appeal theory explain fears and barriers to prenatal physical activity? Womens Health Issues. 2015;25(2):149–154.

62.

Halkjelsvik T, Lund KE, Kraft P, Rise J. Fear appeals in advanced tobacco control environments: the impact of a national mass media campaign in Norway. Health Educ Res. 2013;28(5):888–897.

63.

Witte K, Allen M. A meta-analysis of fear appeals: implications for effective public health campaigns. Health Educ Behav. 2000;27(5):591–615.

64.

Kraus SJ. Attitudes and the prediction of behavior: a meta-analysis of the empirical literature. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 1995;21(1):58–75.

65.

Sheeran P. Intention – behavior relations: a conceptual and empirical review. Eur Rev Soc Psychol. 2002;12(1):1–36.

66.

Ajzen I, Fisbbein M. Factors influencing intentions and the intention-behavior relation. Human Relations. 1974;27(1):1–15.

67.

Rogers RW. A protection motivation theory of fear appeals and attitude change1. J Psychol. 1975;91(1):93–114.

68.

Prochaska JO, Velicer WF. The transtheoretical model of health behavior change. Am J Health Promot. 1997;12(1):38–48.

69.

Montazeri A, Goshtasebi A, Vahdaninia M. Educational inequalities in self-reported health in a general Iranian population. BMC Res Notes. 2008;1(1):50.

70.

Hersch J, Jansen J, Mccaffery K. Decision-making about mammographic screening: pursuing informed choice. Climacteric. 2018;21(3):209–213.

71.

Sicsic J, Pelletier-Fleury N, Moumjid N. Women’s benefits and harms trade-offs in breast cancer screening: results from a discrete-choice experiment. Value Health. 2018;21(1):78–88.

Creative Commons License © 2018 The Author(s). This work is published and licensed by Dove Medical Press Limited. The full terms of this license are available at https://www.dovepress.com/terms.php and incorporate the Creative Commons Attribution - Non Commercial (unported, v3.0) License. By accessing the work you hereby accept the Terms. Non-commercial uses of the work are permitted without any further permission from Dove Medical Press Limited, provided the work is properly attributed. For permission for commercial use of this work, please see paragraphs 4.2 and 5 of our Terms.