Back to Journals » Patient Preference and Adherence » Volume 18

Measuring the Influence of Side Effect Expectations, Beliefs, and Incident Side Effects on the Risk for Drug Discontinuation Among Individuals Starting New Medications, a Cross-sectional Study

Authors Blackburn DF , Yao S, Taylor JG, Alefan Q , Lix LM , Eurich DT , Choudhry NK

Received 21 November 2023

Accepted for publication 4 May 2024

Published 17 May 2024 Volume 2024:18 Pages 979—989

DOI https://doi.org/10.2147/PPA.S451012

Checked for plagiarism Yes

Review by Single anonymous peer review

Peer reviewer comments 2

Editor who approved publication: Dr Johnny Chen



David F Blackburn,1 Shenzhen Yao,2 Jeff G Taylor,1 Qais Alefan,3 Lisa M Lix,4 Dean T Eurich,5 Niteesh K Choudhry6

1College of Pharmacy and Nutrition, University of Saskatchewan, Saskatoon, Saskatchewan, Canada; 2Public Health Surveillance Unit, Vancouver Coastal Health Authority, Vancouver Coastal Health, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada; 3Faculty of Pharmacy, Jordan University of Science and Technology, Irbid, Jordan; 4Department of Community Health Sciences, Max Rady Faculty of Medicine, University of Manitoba, Winnipeg, Manitoba, Canada; 5School of Public Health, University of Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta, Canada; 6Department of Medicine, Brigham and Women’s Hospital/Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA

Correspondence: David F Blackburn, Email [email protected]

Purpose: To measure the impact of beliefs, expectations, side effects, and their combined effects on the risk for medication nonpersistence.
Patients and methods: Using a cross-sectional design, individuals from Saskatchewan, Canada who started a new antihypertensive, cholesterol-lowering, or antihyperglycemic medication were surveyed about risk factors for nonpersistence including: (a) beliefs measured by a composite score of three questions asking about the threat of the condition, importance of the drug, and harm of the drug; (b) incident side effects attributed to treatment; and (c) expectations for side effects before starting treatment. Descriptive statistics and logistic regression models were used to quantify the influence of these risk factors on the outcome of nonpersistence. Odds ratios (ORs) and 95% confidence intervals (CIs) were estimated.
Results: Among 3,029 respondents, 5.8% (n=177) reported nonpersistence within four months after starting the new drug. After adjustment for numerous covariates representing sociodemographics, health-care providers, medication experiences and beliefs, both negative beliefs (OR: 7.26, 95%CI: 4.98– 10.59) and incident side effects (OR: 8.00, 95%CI: 5.49– 11.68) were associated with the highest odds of nonpersistence with no evidence of interaction. In contrast, expectations for side effects before starting treatment exhibited an important interaction with incident side effects following treatment initiation. Among respondents with incident side effects (n=741, 24.5%), the risk for early nonpersistence was 11.5% if they indicated an expectation for side effects before starting the medication compared to 23.6% if they did not (adjusted OR: 0.38, 95%CI: 0.25– 0.60).
Conclusion: Expectations for side effects may be a previously unrecognized but important marker of the probability to persist with treatment. A high percentage of new medication users appeared unprepared for the possibility of side effects from their new medication making them less resilient if side effects occur.

Keywords: adherence, persistence, beliefs, attitudes, expectations

Introduction

Cardiovascular diseases consume a major portion of total health-care expenditures (ie, $350 billion annually in the USA).1 Although increased use of risk-modifying medications has contributed to declining morbidity and health spending over the past two decades, adherence to these medications remains low.2–5 Multiple studies have documented the high prevalence of nonadherence to cardiometabolic medications2–5 resulting in poor risk factor control,6,7 hospitalizations,8,9 and potentially avoidable deaths.10,11 Poor adherence can manifest in different ways. Individuals may skip doses during a course of therapy, often referred to as noncompliance12 or poor implementation.13 Alternatively, individuals may discontinue their drug altogether, an outcome known as nonpersistence.12,13

Nonpersistence to medications often occurs within days to weeks after the first dose.4,14,15 Although side effects are considered a major cause of early nonpersistence,16 the willingness to persist with a new medication is highly influenced by an individual’s assessment of necessity versus concern.17 A minor side effect may be unbearable for a person who believes their drug has few health benefits, while severe side effects may be tolerated by those believing their drug is essential. We previously found that negative medication beliefs were the most powerful predictors of early drug discontinuation among people experiencing side effects.18 That is to say, patients with low confidence in their medication were more likely to discontinue it after a side effect occurred. Notably, expectations and beliefs can also influence the perception of side effects in addition to influencing the response to them.19–23 However, it is unknown whether heightened perceptions of side effects (ie, the nocebo effect) will increase nonpersistence to a greater extent than that expected from negative beliefs alone. The practical relevance of these unresolved issues faces health-care providers daily. It could be argued that a prominent objective of many health-care providers is to inform individuals about side effects that should be expected.24 Our research aim was to measure the impact of negative beliefs, expectations, incidents of side effects, and their combined effects on premature drug discontinuation (ie, nonpersistence).

Methods

Study data were from a population-based questionnaire mailed to residents in the province of Saskatchewan, Canada. The overall aim of the questionnaire was to capture adherence determinants that could be integrated with administrative databases to develop a comprehensive prediction model.18 Saskatchewan has a universal drug insurance program that covers approximately 90% of the population of over one million residents regardless of age or socioeconomic status. All beneficiaries of the provincial drug insurance program were eligible to receive the questionnaire if they were at least 30 years old upon receiving a new claim for an antihypertensive, cholesterol-lowering, or antihyperglycemic medication between September 2019 and February 2020. To restrict the sample to new users only, eligible beneficiaries required at least two years of continuous provincial health coverage prior to the qualifying prescription without any prior claims for these eligible medications.18 Permanent residents of long-term care facilities were not included. Screening for eligible beneficiaries was conducted on several occasions during the two year period; thus, questionnaires were mailed within four to six weeks of the earliest dispensation for one of these eligible medications. The mailout and consent form followed the Dillman strategy including an invitation letter, questionnaire package, and follow-up reminder.25

Participants

Respondents were included in the analysis if they: (a) confirmed receiving one of the eligible medications within the past four months; (b) returned a completed survey with a completed consent to participate; and (c) answered all items relating to persistence (one question), side effects (one question), and medication beliefs (three questions). Questionnaire design and dissemination have been described in detail elsewhere.18 Briefly, the questionnaire contained 58 items designed to collect information on factors influencing adherence during the early phase of treatment including: patient-related factors (eg, knowledge, attitudes, beliefs), social and economic factors (eg, income, marital status), treatment-related factors (eg, side effects), and health-care system factors (eg, appointment length, relationship/trust/support).18

Seven of the questionnaire items were grouped into two composite scores developed previously: a knowledge score and health-care provider support score.18 The first score represented health-care provider support and included five questions for a maximum of 25 points. Questions asked whether respondents were given a chance to ask the doctor questions, received information about side effects, the doctor spent time to help them understand, a nurse or pharmacist spent time, and if they trusted the doctor. Higher scores represented greater support from a health-care provider (Cronbach’s alpha 0.82).18 Also, a knowledge score was calculated from two questions asking whether they knew what the medication was used for and the reasons why the medicine was good for them (Cronbach’s alpha 0.77).18 Both scores were dichotomized into binary variables as described previously.18

Nonpersistence, Incident Side Effects, Expectations for Side Effects, and Beliefs

The primary outcome was nonpersistence, measured with the following question, “Are you still taking the new medicine prescribed to you?” (yes or no). Incidents of side-effects were identified by asking, “Did you experience side effect(s) from your new medicine?” (yes, no, not sure). Expectations for side effects was elicited by asking, “You expected to get side effects from this new medicine before you started taking it” (strongly agree, agree, not sure, disagree, strongly disagree).

Beliefs about medications were measured using three items with 5-point Likert scale response options ranging from strongly agree to strongly disagree. The first question asked about the “perceived threat of the medical condition” (ie, “Your new medicine is for a condition that is a danger to your health”). The second question focused on the “expected importance of the drug” (ie, “You are convinced that your new medicine is important for your health”), and the third asked about “perceived drug harms” (ie, “You worry that your new medicine will do more harm than good”). These questions were based on previously published questionnaires17,26 and demonstrated concordance validity through a strong association with the risk of nonpersistence among a subgroup of respondents who experienced side effects in our previous study.18 We calculated an overall beliefs score for each respondent based on the sum of responses to the three questions.26 The beliefs score produced a total possible score of 15 points and a minimum of three points. Based on the overall beliefs score, individuals were dichotomized into “positive beliefs” and “negative beliefs” using the 25th percentile threshold (ie, score of <11 was considered “negative beliefs”). Cronbach’s alpha27 for the overall beliefs score was 0.68.

Statistical Analysis

We presented characteristics descriptively for the overall sample and for persistent and nonpersistent respondents using frequencies, percentages, means, standard deviations, and medians. Differences in characteristics between those reporting persistence and non-persistence were tested using chi-squared statistics.

Multivariable logistic regression models were used to assess the impact of beliefs, incident side effects, and expectations for side-effects on the outcome of nonpersistence in addition to the entire array of possible adherence predictors in the questionnaire. We fit a series of models in which all variables, other than age and sex, were first tested independently with the outcome and included in the final model only if they reached a significance level P<0.10 on univariate analysis and improved model discrimination performance determined by a statistically significant improvement in the integrated discrimination improvement statistic (IDI).28 The final model also included age greater than 65 years and sex. None of the variables in the final model exhibited multicollinearity defined as a variance inflation factor of 2.5 or greater. We also tested a two-way interaction between side effects and beliefs to identify any statistical evidence of influence between these variables. We then tested the interaction between an “expectation for side effects” and incident side effects. Finally, we repeated the logistic regression analysis using individual beliefs questions rather than the overall beliefs score to ensure our results were not influenced by the decision to represent beliefs with a single score.

A c-statistic (ie, area under the receiver operating characteristic curve) was calculated for each model to assess overall discriminative performance.29 Odds ratios (ORs) and 95% confidence intervals (CIs) were estimated. All analyses were conducted using SAS Software v9.4 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA). Our study was approved by the University of Saskatchewan Committee for Ethics in Human Research and complies with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Results

Out of 11,970 eligible individuals who were mailed a survey, 3,973 responded yielding a response rate of 33.2%. Of these, 3,029 (76.2%) reported starting new antihypertensive, cholesterol-lowering, or antihyperglycemic medication and answered all questions relating to side effects and beliefs (Figure 1). Respondents were equally distributed between males (49.7%) and females (49.5%) and the average age was 62.2 years (SD: 11.7 years). The majority of respondents were Caucasian (89.9%, n=2724), two-thirds had pursued formal education or training beyond high school (63.8%, n=1934), and over three-quarters were married or living with a partner (76.6%, n=2319). Most respondents listed general health status and mental health status as good to excellent (85.0% and 91.1%, respectively), and 82.9% (n=2510) earned more than $25,000 annually (CAD). Lipid-lowering medications were the most common new drug reported (53.5%, n=1620), followed by antihypertensives (28.9%, n=876), and antihyperglycemics (17.6%, n=533). Almost three quarters of patients received the prescription from their “regular doctor” (72.0%, n=2180) and 78.6% (n=2415) were taking two or more medications daily at the time of completing the questionnaire (Table 1).

Table 1 Characteristics of the Study Cohort

Figure 1 Flowchart for selection of study cohort.

Negative beliefs were expressed by only 1.8% to 10.2% of respondents in each of the three beliefs questions. The median overall beliefs score was 12 (range: 3–15) and respondents falling in the lowest quartile corresponded to a score <11 (17.2%, n=522). Incident side effects were reported by 24.5% (n=741). One quarter of respondents (25.8%, n=781) indicated they expected to get side effects from the new medicine before they started taking it.

Five variables were significantly associated with the outcome of nonpersistence based on the univariate model results: negative versus positive beliefs (21.5%, n=112 vs 2.7%, n=67, P<0.001) (Table 2); incident side effects vs no side effects (16.9%, n=125 vs 2.4%, n=54, P<0.001); expecting side effects before starting treatment (vs not expecting), (8.1%, n=63 vs 5.2%, n=116, P=0.032), a low (vs high) knowledge score (less than seven vs higher than six, (8.5%, n=95 vs 4.3%, n=82, P<0.001); and a low health-care provider score (less than 17 vs higher than 16, 8.4%, n=120 vs 3.7%, n=58, P<0.001). The influence of side effects and beliefs on the risk for nonpersistence were consistent regardless of the presence of each other. (Table 2) The highest risk of nonpersistence was observed in people expressing both negative beliefs and side effects at the same time (38.9%, n=82), while the risk for nonpersistence was extremely low in people without either risk factor (1.2%, n=24) (Table 2).

Table 2 Frequency and Percentage of Respondents Expressing Beliefs, Side Effects, and Nonpersistence

Side effects were reported more frequently in respondents with negative beliefs (ie, 40.4% when beliefs score <11 vs 21.1% when score was 11 or higher, P<0.001). Side effects were also reported more commonly by those who expected them before starting the new medication (55.4% vs 14.7%, P<0.001).

Variables entered in the multivariable logistic regression model included age, sex, and the five variables significantly associated with early nonpersistence in univariate models: knowledge score, health-care provider score, expectation for side effects, beliefs, and incident side effects (Table 3). In the final model, three variables were significantly associated with nonpersistence: (1) negative beliefs (ie, overall beliefs score <11): (OR: 7.47, 95%CI: 5.13–10.86); (2) incident side effects: (OR 8.00, 95%CI: 5.49–11.68); and (3) expectations for side-effects before starting treatment: (OR: 0.57, 95%CI: 0.39–0.83).

Table 3 Odds Ratios and 95% Confidence Intervals for Logistic Regression Analysis of Risk Factors Associated with Medication Nonpersistence

No statistically significant interaction was detected between occurrence of side effects and beliefs. However, the two-way interaction between the expectation for side effects and incident side effects was significant; thus, the interaction term was included in the final model. The c-statistic for the final model containing the main effects plus the two-way interaction was 0.84.

Investigation of the significant interaction term revealed a potentially important effect modification. Among respondents with incident side effects, the risk for early nonpersistence was 11.5% if they indicated an expectation for side effects before starting the medication compared to 23.6% if they did not expect side effects prior to starting the drug (adjusted OR: 0.38, 95%CI: 0.25–0.60). A prior expectation for side effects was less important for those who did not experience side effects from their new drug (ie, 4.3 vs 2.0% for those expecting vs not expecting side effects, adjusted OR: 1.59, 95%CI: 0.85–2.98).

Discussion

We conducted a population-based survey study of individuals receiving a new medication from one of three common pharmacologic classes: antihypertensives, cholesterol-lowering, or antihyperglycemics. Negative beliefs and incident side effects were the strongest risk factors for early nonpersistence; their effects were additive but we found no evidence that they influence each other. Individuals with both side effects and negative beliefs exhibited the highest risk for nonpersistence while the absence of both factors virtually eliminated the risk. In contrast to beliefs, an expectation for side effects before starting treatment appeared to protect against the negative influence of incident side effects. Specifically, those who expected side effects were half as likely to discontinue if side effects occurred. However, the majority of respondents expected no side effects before starting their new medication.

People who expect side effects are considered more likely to exhibit negative beliefs about medications and also more likely to experience nocebo effects. A nocebo effects is defined as an “amplified perception of benign sensations” based on knowledge or anxiety about a given drug.19 Those who do not expect side effects are generally considered optimistic and accepting of medication therapy.19,20,22,23,30 Although we found a higher incidence of side effects among those who expected them, the results of our study provide a very different perspective about the potential influence of side effect expectations on the risk for nonpersistence.

According to our results, an expectation for side effects may play a role in preparing some people for tolerability issues during the early stages of therapy. In contrast, expecting no side effects before starting a new medication could be an inconspicuous risk factor for nonpersistence. It is not clear why our results appear to conflict with research about nocebo effects; however, studies examining nocebo effects tended to be performed in highly controlled conditions, often with healthy volunteers responding to hypothetical situations.30 Our population was drawn from real world patients initiating a medication for a chronic condition. Furthermore, knowledge about side effects has been associated with high medication adherence previously,31 and patients have indicated that side effect information is important to help inform their own treatment decisions.32

If side effect expectations do provide some protection against the risk for nonpersistence, it is important to note that only one quarter of our respondents expected side effects before starting their new drug. This result was somewhat unexpected given the widespread public awareness of drug adverse effects33 and attention given to health-care provider communication.24 However, when counseling patients on medication adverse effects, physicians and pharmacists are often focused on reducing fear in order to facilitate positive attitudes.24,34,35 Perhaps these well-intentioned priorities are instilling false expectations among patients that side effects will not occur. Our study suggests that practitioners should ensure patients understand that side effects are a real possibility, which may not be clear if patient education is overly optimistic.

Limitations

We conducted a rigorous analysis of patient-reported risk factors for nonpersistence. However, several limitations must be noted. First, our data was derived from voluntary responders to a population-level study invitation. Thus, we cannot be certain the aggregate experiences collected from our study sample reflect population averages. For example, the low frequency of negative beliefs observed in our sample may have been influenced by a healthy responder bias.36 Despite this limitation, we found clear associations between negative beliefs and side effects with nonpersistence to medications that we believe exist in the general population. Second, our questionnaire was developed for maximum breadth of potential adherence risk factors. To minimize responder fatigue, we collected important factors such as negative beliefs using abbreviated measures in many cases. Our approach produced two important disadvantages, it reduced the potential granularity of our analysis relating to the major sub-scales of the beliefs paradigm (ie, necessity, concern, overuse, and harm);37 it also potentially reduced the validity of the measure given the reliance on a very limited number of questions. However, we had high confidence in the clarity and face validity of our beliefs questions. In addition, the association between our questions about side effects, beliefs, and expectations were very strong and allowed for a clear assessment of impact and interaction with a wide range of other factors. Third, the cross-sectional design of our study made it impossible to confirm temporal relationships between beliefs, starting the new drug, and emergence of side effects. Thus, we can only speculate about the true nature of the associations reported in this study. Fourth and finally, all our data were self-reported. Thus, we cannot verify the accuracy and honesty of respondents to vital questions such as “Are you still taking the medicine prescribed to you” (ie, our primary endpoint). However, we have confidence in the clarity and importance of discontinuing medication as a definitive and relevant outcome.

Conclusion

Prior to starting a new medication, an individual’s expectations for side effects may be a previously unrecognized but important marker of their probability to persist with treatment. A high percentage of new medication users appear unprepared for the possibility of side effects from their new medication making them less resilient if side effects occur.

Acknowledgment

This paper has been uploaded to medRxiv as a preprint: https://www.medrxiv.org/content/10.1101/2023.09.18.23295758v1.

Funding

This study was funded from a peer reviewed research grant from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR grant number 130343). CIHR had no role in the study design, collection, analysis, interpretation of data, writing of the report, or in the decision to submit the article for publication. In addition, all authors had full access to all of the data (including statistical reports and tables) in the study and can take responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. This study is based in part on de-identified data provided by the Saskatchewan Ministry of Health. The interpretation and conclusions contained herein do not necessarily represent those of the Government of Saskatchewan or the Saskatchewan Ministry of Health.

Disclosure

Dr Niteesh Choudhry reports grants from National Institutes of Health, personal fees from RxAnte, outside the submitted work. The authors report no other conflicts of interest in this work.

References

1. Virani SS, Alonso A, Aparicio HJ, et al. Heart disease and stroke statistics—2021 update. Circulation. 2021;143(8):e254–e743. doi:10.1161/CIR.0000000000000950

2. Herkert D, Vijayakumar P, Luo J, et al. Cost-related insulin underuse among patients with diabetes. JAMA Intern Med. 2019;179(1):112–114. doi:10.1001/jamainternmed.2018.5008

3. Mahtta D, Ramsey DJ, Al Rifai M, et al. Evaluation of aspirin and statin therapy use and adherence in patients with premature atherosclerotic cardiovascular disease. JAMA Network Open. 2020;3(8):e2011051–e2011051. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2020.11051

4. Navar AM, Roe MT, Cannon CP, et al. Medication discontinuation in the IMPROVE-IT trial. Circ Cardiovas Qual Outcomes. 2019;12:1–11.

5. Korhonen MJ, Robinson JG, Annis IE, et al. Adherence tradeoff to multiple preventive therapies and all-cause mortality after acute myocardial infarction. J Am Coll Cardiol. 2017;70(13):1543–1554. doi:10.1016/j.jacc.2017.07.783

6. Donnelly LA, Morris AD, Evans JM, Collaboration DM, Evans JMM. Adherence to insulin and its association with glycaemic control in patients with type 2 diabetes. Qjm. 2007;100(6):345–350. doi:10.1093/qjmed/hcm031

7. Krousel-Wood M, Joyce C, Holt E, et al. Predictors of decline in medication adherence. Results from the cohort study of medication adherence among older adults. Hypertension. 2011;58:804–810. doi:10.1161/HYPERTENSIONAHA.111.176859

8. Herttua K, Tabã¡k AG, Martikainen P, Vahtera J, Kivimã¤ki M. Adherence to antihypertensive therapy prior to the first presentation of stroke in hypertensive adults: population-based study. Eur Heart J. 2013;34:2933–2939. doi:10.1093/eurheartj/eht219

9. An J, Nichol MB. Multiple medication adherence and its effect on clinical outcomes among patients with comorbid type 2 diabetes and hypertension. Med Care. 2013;51:879–887. doi:10.1097/MLR.0b013e31829fa8ed

10. Rasmussen JN, Chong A, Alter DA. Relationship between adherence to evidence-based pharmacotherapy and long-term mortality after acute myocardial infarction. JAMA. 2007;297(2):177–186. doi:10.1001/jama.297.2.177

11. Ho PM, Rumsfeld JS, Masoudi FA, et al. Effect of medication nonadherence on hospitalization and mortality among patients with diabetes mellitus. Arch of Int Med. 2006;166(17):1836–1841. doi:10.1001/archinte.166.17.1836

12. Cramer JA, Roy A, Burrell A, et al. Medication compliance and persistence: terminology and definitions. Value Health. 2008;11(1):44–47. doi:10.1111/j.1524-4733.2007.00213.x

13. Vrijens B, De Geest S, Hughes DA, et al. A new taxonomy for describing and defining adherence to medications. Br J Clin Pharmacol. 2012;73(5):691–705. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2125.2012.04167.x

14. Karter AJ, Parker MM, Moffet HH, Ahmed AT, Schmittdiel JA, Selby JV. New prescription medication gaps: a comprehensive measure of adherence to new prescriptions. Health Serv Res. 2009;44(5):1640–1661. doi:10.1111/j.1475-6773.2009.00989.x

15. Evans CD, Eurich DT, Remillard AJ, Shevchuk YM, Blackburn DF. First-fill medication discontinuations and non-adherence to antihypertensive therapy: an observational study. Am J Hypertens. 2012;25:195–203. doi:10.1038/ajh.2011.198

16. Flory JH, Keating SJ, Siscovick D, Mushlin AI. Identifying prevalence and risk factors for metformin non-persistence: a retrospective cohort study using an electronic health record. BMJ open. 2018;8(7):e021505. doi:10.1136/bmjopen-2018-021505

17. Horne R, Weinman J, Hankins M. The beliefs about medicines questionnaire: the development and evaluation of a new method for assessing the cognitive representation of medication. Psychol Health. 1999;14:1–24. doi:10.1080/08870449908407311

18. Alefan Q, Yao S, Taylor JG, et al. Factors associated with early nonpersistence among patients experiencing side effects from a new medication. J Am Pharm Assoc. 2022;62(3):717–726.e715. doi:10.1016/j.japh.2021.12.001

19. Rief W, Avorn J, Barsky AJ. Medication-attributed adverse effects in placebo groups: implications for assessment of adverse effects. Arch Inter Med. 2006;166(2):155–160. doi:10.1001/archinte.166.2.155

20. MacKrill K, Webster R, Rubin GJ, et al. When symptoms become side effects: development of the side effect attribution scale (SEAS). J Psychosom Res. 2021;141:110340. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychores.2020.110340

21. Wood FA, Howard JP, Finegold JA, et al. N-of-1 trial of a statin, placebo, or no treatment to assess side effects. N Engl J Med. 2020;383(22):2182–2184. doi:10.1056/NEJMc2031173

22. Espinola CW, Khoo Y, Parmar R, et al. Pretreatment anxious depression as a predictor of side effect frequency and severity in escitalopram and aripiprazole adjunctive therapy. Brain Behavior. 2022;12(5):e2555. doi:10.1002/brb3.2555

23. Smith LE, Webster RK, Rubin GJ. A systematic review of factors associated with side-effect expectations from medical interventions. Health Expect. 2020;23(4):731–758. doi:10.1111/hex.13059

24. Dyck A, Deschamps M, Taylor J. Pharmacists’ discussions of medication side effects: a descriptive study. Patient Educ Couns. 2005;56(1):21–27. doi:10.1016/j.pec.2003.10.006

25. Dillman DA, Smyth JD, Christian LM. Internet, Mail, and Mixed-Mode: The Tailored Design Method. 3rd ed. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley; 2009.

26. McHorney CA. The adherence estimator: a brief, proximal screener for patient propensity to adhere to prescription medications for chronic disease. Curr Med Res Opin. 2009;25:215–238. doi:10.1185/03007990802619425

27. Bland JM, Altman DG. Cronbach’s alpha. BMJ. 1997;314(7080):572. doi:10.1136/bmj.314.7080.572

28. Pencina MJ, Steverberg EW, D’Agonstino RB. Extensions of net reclassification improvement calculations to measure usefulness of new biomarkers. Stat Med. 2011;30(1):11–21. doi:10.1002/sim.4085

29. Pencina MJ, D’Agonstino RB. Evaluating discrimination of risk prediction models. The C Statistic. JAMA. 2015;314(10):1063–1064. doi:10.1001/jama.2015.11082

30. Webster RK, Rubin GJ. Predicting expectations of side-effects for those which are warned versus not warned about in patient information leaflets. Ann Behav Med. 2021;55(12):1253–1261. doi:10.1093/abm/kaab015

31. Okuyan B, Sancar M, Izzettin FV. Assessment of medication knowledge and adherence among patients under oral chronic medication treatment in community pharmacy settings. Pharmacoepidemiol Drug Saf. 2013;22(2):209–214. doi:10.1002/pds.3275

32. Nair K, Dolovich L, Cassels A, et al. What patients want to know about their medications. Focus group study of patient and clinician perspectives. Cana Famil Phys Med Fame Canad. 2002;48:104–110.

33. Pound P, Britten N, Morgan M, et al. Resisting medicines: a synthesis of qualitative studies of medicine taking. Soc Sci Med. 2005;61(1):133–155. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2004.11.063

34. Kairuz T, Krehula D, Huynh T, Bellamy K. Exploring the process of conveying information about side effects: a qualitative study among pharmacists. Pharmacy. 2013;1(2):256–268. doi:10.3390/pharmacy1020256

35. Salinas GD, Susalka D, Burton BS, et al. Risk assessment and counseling behaviors of healthcare professionals managing patients with chronic pain: a national multifaceted assessment of physicians, pharmacists, and their patients. J Opioid Manag. 2012;8(5):273–284. doi:10.5055/jom.2012.0127

36. Martinson BC, Crain AL, Sherwood NE, Hayes MG, Pronk NP, O’Connor PJ. Population reach and recruitment bias in a maintenance RCT in physically active older adults. J Phys Act Health. 2010;7(1):127–135. doi:10.1123/jpah.7.1.127

37. Horne R, Weinman J. Patients’ beliefs about prescribed medicines and their role in adherence to treatment in chronic physical illness. J Psychosom Res. 1999;47(6):555–567. doi:10.1016/S0022-3999(99)00057-4

Creative Commons License © 2024 The Author(s). This work is published and licensed by Dove Medical Press Limited. The full terms of this license are available at https://www.dovepress.com/terms.php and incorporate the Creative Commons Attribution - Non Commercial (unported, v3.0) License. By accessing the work you hereby accept the Terms. Non-commercial uses of the work are permitted without any further permission from Dove Medical Press Limited, provided the work is properly attributed. For permission for commercial use of this work, please see paragraphs 4.2 and 5 of our Terms.