Back to Journals » Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment » Volume 14

“Low road” to rehabilitation: a perspective on subliminal sensory neuroprosthetics

Authors Ghai S, Ghai I , Effenberg AO

Received 6 October 2017

Accepted for publication 20 November 2017

Published 17 January 2018 Volume 2018:14 Pages 301—307

DOI https://doi.org/10.2147/NDT.S153392

Checked for plagiarism Yes

Review by Single anonymous peer review

Peer reviewer comments 2

Editor who approved publication: Dr Roger Pinder



Shashank Ghai,1 Ishan Ghai,2 Alfred O Effenberg1

1Institute of Sports Science, Leibniz University Hannover, Hannover, 2School of Life Sciences, Jacobs University, Bremen, Germany

Abstract: Fear can propagate parallelly through both cortical and subcortical pathways. It can instigate memory consolidation habitually and might allow internal simulation of movements independent of the cortical structures. This perspective suggests delivery of subliminal, aversive and kinematic audiovisual stimuli via neuroprosthetics in patients with neocortical dysfunctions. We suggest possible scenarios by which these stimuli might bypass damaged neocortical structures and possibly assisting in motor relearning. Anticipated neurophysiological mechanisms and methodological scenarios have been discussed in this perspective. This approach introduces novel perspectives into neuropsychology as to how subcortical pathways might be used to induce motor relearning.

Keywords: motor learning, fear perception, internal simulation, sonification, cortical dysfunctions

Background

The structural organization of a human brain is like a mushroom growing inside out, suggesting the ancient prevalence of innermost subcortical structures such as brain stem, amygdala to superficial neocortical structure such as prefrontal cortex. Evolution has bestowed different functional roles on these neural centers based on their development; for instance, the innermost structures usually mediate basic survival functions, such as breathing and fear (threat) processing, whereas the outermost structures manage sophisticated abilities such as decision-making and self-control and more.1 Being a basic survival function, fear is mainly mediated within the innermost, subcortical structures of the brain.13 However, due to the evolutionary course, neocortical structures have also formed parallel connections for processing fear, possibly to allow a more cognitive and context-driven processing of the stimuli.35 LeDoux4 labeled such parallel processing of fear by subcortical pathways as “low road processing” and cortical pathways as “high road processing”. However, these pathways operate on distinct terms. On one hand, the “low road” pathways process stimuli in a “quick and dirty” manner while utilizing subcortical pathways, and independent of consciousness.6,7 This pathway prioritizes physical safety and acts as a fail-safe mechanism while ignoring any social or environmental context whatsoever. On the other hand, the “high road” pathways allow a rather slower resource-dependent cognitive processing of stimuli via higher cortical structures and prioritize contextual information associated with social, psychological and environmental factors. For instance, longer propagation latency has been reported when fear processing takes place through higher cortical structures, possibly suggesting costs for higher level processing,8 whereas processing with “low road pathways” has been reported to be considerably shorter, ie, as low as 30–120 ms.9 Neuroanatomical studies reveal that processing of stimuli through “low road” allows the propagation of fear stimuli in amygdala by the way of superior colliculi and pulvinar nuclei of thalamus,4,10 a short pathway, whereas in the high road pathway, for visual information, the stimuli would pass from the retinal ganglion cells to lateral geniculate nucleus, visual cortex (V1, V2 and V4) and inferior temporal cortex, and then end up in amygdala. Under the conditions of threat, mediation of stimuli first to the “low road” pathway is gated by amygdala,8 for both visual11,12 and auditory streams.13,14 It might be because of its higher sensitivity to process low spatial frequency information,15,16 thereby initiating action even to a “close enough” stimulus.6 For instance, Carter and Frith5 proposed that parallel processing by high and low roads17 allows mediating balance between cortex and the amygdala by allowing both contextualized and fail-safe responses to a threat, respectively.

Several cortical and subcortical structures take part in processing fear-related stimuli. For instance, hypothalamus, amygdala, superior colliculi, lateral geniculate nuclei, thalamus (pulvinar nuclei), locus coeruleus and periaqueductal gray are the main subcortical structures involved in mediating fear,10,18 whereas (medial-lateral) prefrontal, orbitofrontal, visual, parietal cortices, anterior cingulate cortex and hippocampus and bilateral anterior insulate cortex are the main cortical structures.8,18 Moreover, the functioning of “low road” subcortical pathways is suggested to be independent of higher cortical processing. For instance, diffusion tensor imaging has demonstrated projections between superior colliculi and amygdala via the pulvinar.19 Furthermore, Morris et al20 in their neuroimaging study reported perception of aversive visual stimuli in a patient with effective blind sight (extensive lesion in occipital cortex).21,22

Additionally, “low road” pathways possess specialized interconnections with the motor control centers of the brain, independent of cortical control, primarily to initiate fight or flight response to a threat. Grezes et al23 using diffusion tensor magnetic resonance imaging and probabilistic tractography demonstrated interconnectivity of amygdala to descending corticospinal tracts, lateral and medial precentral, motor cingulate, primary motor cortices and postcentral gyrus. Gokdemir et al24 further reported fear potentiation of both corticospinal and reticulospinal pathways in humans, post auditory and visual fear conditioning. Moreover, a strong role of these primitive subcortical pathways has also been reported for the perception of biological motion.25,26 Furl et al27 in an fMRI analysis revealed enhanced fear sensitivity in dorsal and ventral temporal motion-sensitive areas corresponding to superior temporal sulcus, hMT+/V5, inferior frontal gyrus, fusiform cortex (fusiform face area) and the action observation system.28 The authors further added that amygdala might also control encoding and prediction of aversive incidence based on the elements of stimuli. Moreover, Bastiaansen et al29 added that such interconnections of amygdala with these motor centers might be helpful in triggering for mirroring of emotions.

Likewise, this subcortical pathway (especially amygdala2) mediates a unique learning and memory mechanism. This mechanism has been reported to play a key role in predicting threat-based events before recognition of sensory stimuli.2,30 Here, amygdala has also been reported to facilitate learning in a rapid,31 habitual1,3134 and resilient manner.35 Possibly, by modulating the activity and connectivity of prefrontal cortex,36,37 Schwabe et al38 suggested that threat-induced stress can selectively gate memory consolidation in favor of thalamus-dependent habitual learning2,39 as compared to hippocampus.33,35 Shiromani et al31 too affirmed that the altered strength of synaptic signaling in amygdala is the major reason for habitual consolidation of memory. The authors stated that relatively weak conditioned stimuli (activating postsynaptic N-methyl D-aspartate receptors) gets strengthened by co-occurrence of unconditioned stimuli (triggering calcium influx), thereby eliciting robust responses in lateral nucleus. Moreover, the independence of this specialized memory system from cortical pathways and resilience in terms of long-term retention have also been reported (thalamo-amygdala pathways7). For instance, Maren and Quirk2 reported lateral amygdala-associated memory plasticity during auditory fear conditioning, even in the presence of large lesions in auditory cortex.40 Nevertheless, despite extensive research confirming the unique ability of the “low road” pathway to govern motor action, perception and memory consolidation independent of cortical structures, its possible role in enhancing prognosis in cases of neocortical dysfunctions has never been discussed in the literature.

As mentioned earlier, neocortex, the outermost and latest evolutionary development of brain, accounts for ~76% of the brain volume.41 Any superficial damage to these structures in cases of trauma and cerebrovascular accidents might cause a wide array of cognitive4244 and sensory–motor dysfunctions.45 Such damages together inflict debilitating symptoms on both cognitive and motor domains, thereby adversely impacting the prognosis of such patients. For instance, damage to prefrontal cortex (dysexecutive syndrome46) might considerably impair conscious perception;47 self-control; task purportedly measuring fluency; concept formation; set shifting; inhibition; attention organization; abstract reasoning; novel problem-solving ability; stimuli inferencing decision-making ability; ability to encode task relevant information in working memory;48,49 ability to select, monitor, manipulate and access current task information44 and others. 50 Shumway-Cook and Woollacott51 suggested that such deficits in attention, working memory allocation and short-term memory might considerably prolong the prognosis in a rehabilitation protocol, where explicit instructions are mainly emphasized.52,53 In this study, we attempt to explain how the specialized abilities of these “low road” pathways could be exploited to enhance motor relearning for aiding in rehabilitation independent of such higher cortical functioning.

Accessing the “low” roads: the novel strategy

In this article, we attempt to suggest possible strategies that could be used to access the subcortical “low road” routes of the brain to facilitate or stimulate the damaged or dormant structures of the brain and aid in rehabilitation. We suggest utilizing task-specific multimodal neuroprosthetics to deliver aversive sensory stimuli subliminally to enhance motor perception and facilitate the process of motor relearning.54 Real-time kinematic auditory feedback (sonification) and kinematic visual feedback generated in some of the widely researched rehabilitation approaches which allow comprehensive and efficient multisensory integration.55,56 Kinematic auditory feedback is a relatively new interdisciplinary approach which has been utilized and demonstrated to enhance motor perception, motor control and learning in rehabilitation.57,58 This methodology takes advantage of the strong relationship between auditory perception and motor control,5962 and has been reported to trigger neural centers associated with biological motion perception.63,64 Also, sonification might provide valuable assistance toward enhancing movement perception of motor patterns associated with/without expertise, further aiding in enhancing representation and internal simulation of a motor task in the action observation system.65,66

Likewise, virtual reality is effective in rehabilitation.67 The environment designed in virtual reality can be customized very similar to real-life settings68 and can possess benefits in terms of transmitting kinematic visual stimuli for augmenting the brain functions by enhancing motor perception,69 especially related to biological motion perception.70 Moreover, the sensorimotor lability of both kinematic auditory and visual stimuli can be used to induce a compelling sense of immersion even when sensory inputs are incongruent and below the conscious threshold.69 Therefore, coupling the use of methodologies can possibly provide opportunities to deliver multimodal multisensory information in terms of kinematic auditory and visual information concomitantly.58,64,65,71 These methodologies have demonstrated to enhance perception,64 efficient human behavior,68,72 motor learning,64 relearning64 and performance,73 thereby allowing benefits in the due course of rehabilitation. Radiological evidence by Schmitz et al64 demonstrated robust activation of a specialized mirror–neuron system and human action observation system, precisely the activation of cortical: superior temporal sulcus, Brodmann’s area 45, 6, and subcortical areas comprising striato-thalamo-frontal motor loop, ie, caudate nucleus, putamen and thalamus. The authors further speculated that such an activation of the action observation system while listening to motor activities might lead to an internal stimulation of perceived movement. Therefore, suggesting an association for increase in mental, auditory imagery.55

Utilizing such multisensory modalities for transmitting aversive subliminal stimuli might allow multifaceted benefits in perceptual domain, for instance, providing kinematic stimuli associated with fearful postures. Supposedly, a wild environment could be generated where a distant predator or imminent danger leads the person to choose a flight response and run away from the situation. Here, the patient could either be subjected to a first person or a third person view i.e., patient perceiving the threat on themselves or on a virtual avatar, respectively. This difference could be selected based on the level of cognitive and meta-cognitive dysfunctions. Further, coupling the audiovisual kinematic information for fearful postures and locomotion might instigate similar changes in the patient’s action observation system and enhance internal simulation associated with locomotion for a “flight” response. For instance, Johansson74 suggested that higher cortical centers are not the main components for perceiving basal biological motion, and therefore, this approach might be efficient in the condition of no-cortical dysfunction. Moreover, the stimuli might also be used to instigate reflexive behavior. For instance, Tamietto and De Gelder75 suggested a strong relationship between the motor domain and amygdala while processing fearful stimuli to elicit reflexive behavior. In this study, we again suggest to possibly exploit this strong network and utilize multisensory integration modalities to address the deficits in motor execution. For instance, virtual reality can be used to generate a specific environment where a predator, such as a snake, tries to attack an extremity, eliciting a reflexive withdrawal reflex. Sonification in such a strategy can be used to superimpose on the executed reflexive action, for instance, aversive auditory feedback can be superimposed on the elbow imitating a flexor withdrawal reflex. Although due to motor restrictions these movements might not be physically executable, simulating these motor movements might allow preemptive facilitation (feed-forward manner) essential for execution.76

Such internal representations should elicit internal representations of motor tasks and thereafter aid in kinesthetic motor imagery for the perceived movement pattern. Moreover, facilitation of neural pathways might also be elicited as a rehabilitation perspective neural pathway for motor execution and imagery, and actively executed motions share a similar neural circuitry.77 Ietswaart et al78 suggested that enhanced brain plasticity because of mental practice can play a very important role in recovery following brain damage. Precisely, imagining or practicing movements could stimulate restitution and redistribution of brain activity, which can enhance the recovery of motor functions (refer “Hebbian theory”79). This when superimposed with conventional passive and active movements by a physiotherapist might provide additional benefits for relearning and performance.8082 Although highly speculative the fearful stimuli provided with biological motion might also instigate memory consolidation of movement patterns in a habitual manner, which in rehabilitation and performance settings have been demonstrated to be extremely beneficial.8387

Moreover, to avoid the detrimental perceptual repercussions in behavior, the stimuli can be delivered subliminally. Perception of fear stimuli has been reportedly maintained even when a stimulus is masked,88 with dichoptic stimulation,89 when stimulus is presented at thresholds90 and in the peripheral vision.91,92 Additionally, visual activation of invisible stimuli can also be strong, when the invisibility is induced by neglect93 or inattention.94 Dehaene et al95 suggested a state of contrast between subliminal and preconscious processing, which possibly could be an appropriate tool or the application of audiovisual stimuli, ie, masking of stimuli combined with inattentiveness. The author implied that within the conscious perception, a subject would be able to recognize and identify the presented stimuli.8 On the contrary, the preconscious state of perception implies that the subject has a relatively strong neural response to the presentation, but either is not yet consciously aware or will miss it due to the absence of attention.95 Finally, we hypothesize this methodological approach to attain perceptual and learning benefits by two mechanisms: first, by eliciting reflexive mechanisms in patients and activating dormant or damaged cortical pathways. Furthermore, this approach can be allocated with activities of daily living, where certain activities can be coupled with aversive sensory inputs. Together they are hypothesized to enhance biological motion perception, higher neural center activation, mental practice, cortical restructuring and regeneration and when coupled with physical therapy, they can lead to additional motor activity in terms of rehabilitative benefits. This perspective for the first time proposes the utilization of “low” road pathways for facilitating higher neocortical structures in case of damage. This approach could also have applications for patients in minimal conscious states where prognosis is exceptionally poor.96 These patients exhibit characteristics similar to higher order cortical dysfunctions.97,98 Additionally, the patients under minimal conscious states as per the categorization by Giacino et al99 and Vincent98 exhibit reproducible visual fixation, emotional and motor behavior. Producing reflexive motor actions via multisensory integration of aversive stimuli can allow the development of increased awareness and elicit neural reorganization. Finally, the main aim of this perspective is to elicit a scientific discussion on the topic, and we strongly urge future studies to analyze this gap in the literature.

As a future prospect, we would like to propose utilization of aversive olfactory stimuli as a possible medium in multisensory integration for enhancing fear perception. Studies have reported the effects olfactory stimuli possess on motor control of human body.100102 Sakamoto et al102 speculated that olfaction possibly could have enhanced stability and motor performance by activating the insular cortex. Similarly, a multisensory integration pattern has been demonstrated in studies evaluating audio-olfactory domain103 and visuo-auditory domain.104 Nonetheless, the most important aspect why we are interested in incorporating olfaction in multisensory integration is its association with the limbic system. Baars and Gage1 suggested that the afferent signals to amygdala arrive via four main pathways. However, the information drawn from olfactory stimuli is perpetuated directly at amygdala from the olfactory cortex without preprocessing at the thalamus, thereby suggesting a profound ability of odor as compared to other sensory stimuli on emotional consolidation of memories. Likewise, the findings of De Groot et al105 are also important where olfactory fear stimuli were described to be as potent as audiovisual fear signals in inducing fear. This could considerably add toward the development of a comprehensive environment to elicit a fear response. Not only this but recent research by Jacobs et al106 have also confirmed the presence of spatial coding information with high precision with olfaction in humans. These findings considerably add toward the prospective use of olfaction with movement perception and virtual reality where the spatial information about the motor movements derived from sensory inputs is a key component.107 Nonetheless, the concept of utilization of olfaction as a possible medium of multisensory integration in movement perception is rather new and has been never discussed in published literature earlier. Recent advancements in virtual reality domain by coupling olfactory inputs by Ubisoft can possibly ascertain future application. Gaming modalities such as Nosulus rift can precisely incorporate aversive scents and couple them in a simulated environment providing enhanced perception benefits. This has been previously described by Richard et al.108 Additionally, we would also suggest utilization of modern neuroprosthetics such as smart skins to enhance afferent inputs from skin receptors to aid in multisensory integration, and relearning.109

Summary

In this article, we propose a possible methodological approach which utilizes the “low” road fear pathways in rehabilitation of neurological disorders characterized by cortical damage primarily leading to executive dysfunctions. Based on the previous findings, this article bridges the published empirical findings and suggests that perception of fear can occur without consciousness. The article also proposes a methodological approach by using multisensory integration modalities, such as real-time kinematic auditory feedback, virtual reality to transfer aversive stimuli via audiovisual input, without conscious awareness to enhance biological motion perception, associated with activities of daily living to enhance mental imagery, practice, preparedness and possibly neural regeneration. Moreover, we also discuss possibly eliciting reflexive motor actions incurred by an aversive stimulus to enhance motor relearning. This coupled with physical rehabilitation can allow more benefits in terms of prognosis. This methodological perspective is aimed to address the poor prognosis faced by patients suffering from neocortical dysfunctions.

Acknowledgments

This article was funded by the open access funds of Leibniz University Hannover. The authors thank Prof Robert Isler and Prof Thomas Münte for their constructive comments and guidance.

Author contributions

Shashank Ghai conceptualized the perspective and wrote the article. Ishan Ghai and Alfred O Effenberg provided useful discussions and reviewed the paper. All authors contributed toward data analysis, drafting and critically revising the paper and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

Disclosure

The authors report no conflicts of interest in this work.


References

1.

Baars BJ, Gage NM. Cognition, Brain, and Consciousness: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience. 2nd ed. Amsterdam: Elsevier/Academic Press; 2010.

2.

Maren S, Quirk GJ. Neuronal signalling of fear memory. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2004;5:844.

3.

Carr JA. I’ll take the low road: the evolutionary underpinnings of visually triggered fear. Front Neurosci. 2015;9:414.

4.

LeDoux J. The Emotional Brain: The Mysterious Underpinnings of Emotional Life. New York, NY: Simon and Schuster; 1998.

5.

Carter R, Frith C. Mapping the Brain. London: Weidenfeld & Nicolson; 1998.

6.

Ohman A, Carlsson K, Lundqvist D, Ingvar M. On the unconscious subcortical origin of human fear. Physiol Behav. 2007;92(1–2):180–185.

7.

Méndez-Bértolo C, Moratti S, Toledano R, et al. A fast pathway for fear in human amygdala. Nat Neurosci. 2016;19:1041.

8.

Silverstein D, Ingvar M. A multi-pathway hypothesis for human visual fear signaling. Front Syst Neurosci. 2015;9:101.

9.

Luo Q, Holroyd T, Majestic C, Cheng X, Schechter J, Blair RJ. Emotional automaticity is a matter of timing. J Neurosci. 2010;30(17):5825–5829.

10.

Pessoa L, Adolphs R. Emotion processing and the amygdala: from a “low road” to “many roads” of evaluating biological significance. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2010;11(11):773–783.

11.

Leopold DA, Logothetis NK. Activity changes in early visual cortex reflect monkeys’ percepts during binocular rivalry. Nature. 1996;379(6565):549.

12.

Mitchell DG, Greening SG. Conscious perception of emotional stimuli: brain mechanisms. Neuroscientist. 2012;18(4):386–398.

13.

Weinberger NM. The medial geniculate, not the amygdala, as the root of auditory fear conditioning. Hear Res. 2011;274(1–2):61–74.

14.

LeDoux JE. Brain mechanisms of emotion and emotional learning. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 1992;2(2):191–197.

15.

Vuilleumier P, Armony JL, Driver J, Dolan RJ. Distinct spatial frequency sensitivities for processing faces and emotional expressions. Nat Neurosci. 2003;6(6):624–631.

16.

de Gelder B, van Honk J, Tamietto M. Emotion in the brain: of low roads, high roads and roads less travelled. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2011;12(7):425.

17.

Day-Brown JD, Wei H, Chomsung RD, Petry HM, Bickford ME. Pulvinar projections to the striatum and amygdala in the tree shrew. Front Neuroanat. 2010;4:143.

18.

Fullana MA, Harrison BJ, Soriano-Mas C, et al. Neural signatures of human fear conditioning: an updated and extended meta-analysis of fMRI studies. Mol Psychiatry. 2016;21(4):500–508.

19.

Tamietto M, Pullens P, de Gelder B, Weiskrantz L, Goebel R. Subcortical connections to human amygdala and changes following destruction of the visual cortex. Curr Biol. 2012;22(15):1449–1455.

20.

Morris JS, DeGelder B, Weiskrantz L, Dolan RJ. Differential extrageniculostriate and amygdala responses to presentation of emotional faces in a cortically blind field. Brain. 2001;124(Pt 6):1241–1252.

21.

Pegna AJ, Khateb A, Lazeyras F, Seghier ML. Discriminating emotional faces without primary visual cortices involves the right amygdala. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8(1):24–25.

22.

Bertini C, Cecere R, Làdavas E. I am blind, but I “see” fear. Cortex. 2013;49(4):985–993.

23.

Grezes J, Valabregue R, Gholipour B, Chevallier C. A direct amygdala-motor pathway for emotional displays to influence action: a diffusion tensor imaging study. Hum Brain Mapp. 2014;35(12):5974–5983.

24.

Gokdemir S, Gunduz A, Ozkara C, Kiziltan ME. Fear-conditioned alterations of motor cortex excitability: the role of amygdala. Neurosci Lett. 2017;662:346–350.

25.

Bonda E, Petrides M, Ostry D, Evans A. Specific involvement of human parietal systems and the amygdala in the perception of biological motion. J Neurosci. 1996;16(11):3737–3744.

26.

De Gelder B, Snyder J, Greve D, Gerard G, Hadjikhani N. Fear fosters flight: a mechanism for fear contagion when perceiving emotion expressed by a whole body. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004;101(47):16701–16706.

27.

Furl N, Henson RN, Friston KJ, Calder AJ. Top-down control of visual responses to fear by the amygdala. J Neurosci. 2013;33(44):17435–17443.

28.

van der Gaag C, Minderaa RB, Keysers C. Facial expressions: what the mirror neuron system can and cannot tell us. Soc Neurosci. 2007;2(3–4):179–222.

29.

Bastiaansen JACJ, Thioux M, Keysers C. Evidence for mirror systems in emotions. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2009;364(1528):2391–2404.

30.

Dolan RJ. Emotion, cognition, and behavior. Science. 2002;298(5596):1191–1194.

31.

Shiromani P, Keane TM, LeDoux JE. Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder. Berlin: Springer; 2014.

32.

Schwabe L, Wolf OT. Stress prompts habit behavior in humans. J Neurosci. 2009;29(22):7191–7198.

33.

Phelps EA. Human emotion and memory: interactions of the amygdala and hippocampal complex. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2004;14(2):198–202.

34.

McGaugh JL. The amygdala modulates the consolidation of memories of emotionally arousing experiences. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2004;27:1–28.

35.

Yonelinas AP, Ritchey M. The slow forgetting of emotional episodic memories: an emotional binding account. Trends Cogn Sci. 2015;19(5):259–267.

36.

Oei NY, Elzinga BM, Wolf OT, et al. Glucocorticoids decrease hippocampal and prefrontal activation during declarative memory retrieval in young men. Brain Imaging Behav. 2007;1(1–2):31–41.

37.

Schwabe L, Tegenthoff M, Höffken O, Wolf OT. Concurrent glucocorticoid and noradrenergic activity shifts instrumental behavior from goal-directed to habitual control. J Neurosci. 2010;30(24):8190–8196.

38.

Schwabe L, Oitzl MS, Philippsen C, et al. Stress modulates the use of spatial versus stimulus-response learning strategies in humans. Learn Mem. 2007;14(1–2):109–116.

39.

Seger CA, Spiering BJ. A critical review of habit learning and the basal ganglia. Front Syst Neurosci. 2011;5(Preprint):66.

40.

Romanski LM, LeDoux JE. Equipotentiality of thalamo-amygdala and thalamo-cortico-amygdala circuits in auditory fear conditioning. J Neurosci. 1992;12(11):4501–4509.

41.

Noback CR, Strominger NL, Demarest RJ, Ruggiero DA. The Human Nervous System: Structure and Function. Berlin: Springer Science & Business Media; 2005.

42.

Castan E, Whishaw IQ, Robinson TE. Recovery from lateralized neocortical damage: dissociation between amphetamine-induced asymmetry in behavior and striatal dopamine neurotransmission in vivo. Brain Res. 1992;571(2):248–259.

43.

McAllister TW. Neurobiological consequences of traumatic brain injury. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2011;13(3):287–300.

44.

Szczepanski SM, Knight RT. Insights into human behavior from lesions to the prefrontal cortex. Neuron. 2014;83(5):1002–1018.

45.

Jahanshahi M. Willed action and its impairments. Cogn Neuropsychol. 1998;15(6–8):483–533.

46.

Baddeley A, Wilson B. Frontal amnesia and the dysexecutive syndrome. Brain Cogn. 1988;7(2):212–230.

47.

Libedinsky C, Livingstone M. Role of prefrontal cortex in conscious visual perception. J Neurosci. 2011;31(1):64–69.

48.

Riley MR, Constantinidis C. Role of prefrontal persistent activity in working memory. Front Syst Neurosci. 2016;9:181.

49.

Lara AH, Wallis JD. The role of prefrontal cortex in working memory: a mini review. Front Syst Neurosci. 2015;9:173.

50.

Mansouri FA, Koechlin E, Rosa MGP, Buckley MJ. Managing competing goals – a key role for the frontopolar cortex. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2017;18:645.

51.

Shumway-Cook A, Woollacott MH. Motor Control: Translating Research into Clinical Practice. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins; 2007.

52.

Shallice T, Burgess PW. Deficits in strategy application following frontal lobe damage in man. Brain. 1991;114(Pt 2):727–741.

53.

Ghai S, Ghai I, Effenberg AO. Effects of dual tasks and dual-task training on postural stability: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Clin Interv Aging. 2017;12:557.

54.

Moulton PM. A motor relearning program for stroke, 2nd edition – Carr JH, Shepherd RB. Am J Occup Therapy. 1989;43(6):418–419.

55.

Sigrist R, Rauter G, Riener R, Wolf P. Augmented visual, auditory, haptic, and multimodal feedback in motor learning: a review. Psychon Bull Rev. 2013;20(1):21–53.

56.

Dascal J, Reid M, IsHak WW, et al. Virtual reality and medical inpatients: a systematic review of randomized, controlled trials. Innov Clin Neurosci. 2017;14(1–2):14–21.

57.

Effenberg AO. Movement sonification: effects on perception and action. IEEE Multimedia. 2005;12(2):53–59.

58.

Dubus G, Bresin R. A systematic review of mapping strategies for the sonification of physical quantities. PLoS One. 2013;8(12):e82491.

59.

Gibet S. Sensorimotor control of sound-producing gestures, musical gestures – sound, movement, and meaning. In: Godoy, Inge R, Leman M (editors). Musical Gestures: Sound, Movement, and Meaning, Routledge. 2009;212–237.

60.

Ghai S, Ghai I, Effenberg AO. Effect of rhythmic auditory cueing on aging gait: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Aging Dis. 2017;131–200.

61.

Ghai S, Ghai I, Effenberg AO. Effect of rhythmic auditory cueing on gait in cerebral palsy: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2018;14:43–59.

62.

Ghai S, Ghai I, Schmitz G, Effenberg AO. Effect of rhythmic auditory cueing on parkinsonian gait: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Scientific reports. In press 2018.

63.

Scheef L, Boecker H, Daamen M, et al. Multimodal motion processing in area V5/MT: evidence from an artificial class of audio-visual events. Brain Res. 2009;1252:94–104.

64.

Schmitz G, Mohammadi B, Hammer A, et al. Observation of sonified movements engages a basal ganglia frontocortical network. BMC Neurosci. 2013;14(1):1.

65.

Effenberg AO, Fehse U, Schmitz G, Krueger B, Mechling H. Movement sonification: effects on motor learning beyond rhythmic adjustments. Front Neurosci. 2016;10:219.

66.

Effenberg AO. Sensory systems: auditory, tactile, proprioceptive. In: Eklund RC, Tenenbaum G, editors. Encyclopedia of Sport and Exercise Psychology. Vol. 2. Los Angeles, CA: SAGE Publications; 2014:663–667.

67.

Rizzo AA, Schultheis M, Kerns KA, Mateer C. Analysis of assets for virtual reality applications in neuropsychology. Neuropsychol Rehabil. 2004;14(1–2):207–239.

68.

Sveistrup H. Motor rehabilitation using virtual reality. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2004;1(1):1.

69.

Wright WG. Using virtual reality to augment perception, enhance sensorimotor adaptation, and change our minds. Front Syst Neurosci. 2014;8:56.

70.

Bouquet C, Gaurier V, Shipley T, Toussaint L, Blandin Y. Influence of the perception of biological or non-biological motion on movement execution. J Sports Sci. 2007;25(5):519–530.

71.

Effenberg A, Fehse U, Weber A. Movement Sonification: audiovisual benefits on motor learning. BIO Web of Conferences. 2011;1.

72.

Butler AJ, James KH. Active learning of novel sound-producing objects: motor reactivation and enhancement of visuo-motor connectivity. J Cogn Neurosci. 2013;25(2):203–218.

73.

Boyer E. Continuous Auditory Feedback for Sensorimotor Learning. Paris: Université Pierre et Marie Curie-Paris VI; 2015.

74.

Johansson G. Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis. Percept Psychophys. 1973;14(2):201–211.

75.

Tamietto M, De Gelder B. Neural bases of the non-conscious perception of emotional signals. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2010;11(10):697.

76.

Rohde M, Di Luca M, Ernst MO. The rubber hand illusion: feeling of ownership and proprioceptive drift do not go hand in hand. PLoS One. 2011;6(6):e21659.

77.

Kuhtz-Buschbeck J, Mahnkopf C, Holzknecht C, Siebner H, Ulmer S, Jansen O. Effector-independent representations of simple and complex imagined finger movements: a combined fMRI and TMS study. Eur J Neurosci. 2003;18(12):3375–3387.

78.

Ietswaart M, Johnston M, Dijkerman HC, et al. Mental practice with motor imagery in stroke recovery: randomized controlled trial of efficacy. Brain. 2011;134(Pt 5):1373–1386.

79.

Hebb DO. The Organization of Behavior: A Neuropsychological Theory. New York, NY: Psychology Press; 2005.

80.

Horki P, Bauernfeind G, Klobassa DS, et al. Detection of mental imagery and attempted movements in patients with disorders of consciousness using EEG. Front Hum Neurosci. 2014;8:1009.

81.

Ghai S, Driller MW, Masters RS. The influence of below-knee compression garments on knee-joint proprioception. Gait Posture. 2016;pii:S0966-6362(16)30484-2.

82.

Ghai S, Driller M, Ghai. Effects of joint stabilizers on proprioception and stability: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Phys Ther Sport. 2017;25:65–75.

83.

Masters RS. Theoretical aspects of implicit learning in sport. Int J Sport Psychol. 2000;31(4):530–541.

84.

Masters RSW. Knowledge, knerves and know-how: the role of explicit versus implicit knowledge in the breakdown of a complex motor skill under pressure. Br J Psychol. 1992;83(3):343–358.

85.

Masters RSW, Maxwell J. The theory of reinvestment. Int Rev Sport Exercise Psychol. 2008;1(2):160–183.

86.

Masters RSW, Poolton JM, Maxwell JP. Stable implicit motor processes despite aerobic locomotor fatigue. Conscious Cogn. 2008;17(1):335–338.

87.

Masters RSW, Poolton JM, Maxwell JP, Raab M. Implicit motor learning and complex decision making in time-constrained environments. J Mot Behav. 2008;40(1):71–79.

88.

Dehaene S, Naccache L, Cohen L, et al. Cerebral mechanisms of word masking and unconscious repetition priming. Nat Neurosci. 2001;4(7):752–758.

89.

Moutoussis K, Zeki S. The relationship between cortical activation and perception investigated with invisible stimuli. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2002;99(14):9527–9532.

90.

Ress D, Heeger DJ. Neuronal correlates of perception in early visual cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2003;6(4):414–420.

91.

Bayle DJ, Henaff M-A, Krolak-Salmon P. Unconsciously perceived fear in peripheral vision alerts the limbic system: a MEG study. PLoS One. 2009;4(12):e8207.

92.

Almeida I, Soares SC, Castelo-Branco M. The distinct role of the amygdala, superior colliculus and pulvinar in processing of central and peripheral snakes. PLoS One. 2015;10(6):e0129949.

93.

Vuilleumier P, Sagiv N, Hazeltine E, et al. Neural fate of seen and unseen faces in visuospatial neglect: a combined event-related functional MRI and event-related potential study. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2001;98(6):3495–3500.

94.

Marois R, Yi D-J, Chun MM. The neural fate of consciously perceived and missed events in the attentional blink. Neuron. 2004;41(3):465–472.

95.

Dehaene S, Changeux J-P, Naccache L, Sackur J, Sergent C. Conscious, preconscious, and subliminal processing: a testable taxonomy. Trends Cogn Sci. 2006;10(5):204–211.

96.

Monti MM, Vanhaudenhuyse A, Coleman MR, et al. Willful modulation of brain activity in disorders of consciousness. N Engl J Med. 2010;362(7):579–589.

97.

Plum F, Posner JB. The Diagnosis of Stupor and Coma. Vol. 19. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 1982.

98.

Vincent J-L. Yearbook of Intensive Care and Emergency Medicine 2002. Berlin: Springer Science & Business Media; 2013.

99.

Giacino J, Zasler N, Whyte J, Katz D, Glen M, Andary M. Recommendations for use of uniform nomenclature pertinent to patients with severe alterations in consciousness. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 1995;76(2):205–209.

100.

Freeman S, Ebihara S, Ebihara T, et al. Olfactory stimuli and enhanced postural stability in older adults. Gait Posture. 2009;29(4):658–660.

101.

Ebihara S, Nikkuni E, Ebihara T, Sakamoto Y, Freeman S, Kohzuki M. Effects of olfactory stimulation on gait performance in frail older adults. Geriatr Gerontol Int. 2012;12(3):567–568.

102.

Sakamoto Y, Ebihara S, Ebihara T, et al. Fall prevention using olfactory stimulation with lavender odor in elderly nursing home residents: a randomized controlled trial. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2012;60(6):1005–1011.

103.

Wesson DW, Wilson DA. Smelling sounds: olfactory–auditory sensory convergence in the olfactory tubercle. J Neurosci. 2010;30(8):3013–3021.

104.

Gottfried JA, Dolan RJ. The nose smells what the eye sees: crossmodal visual facilitation of human olfactory perception. Neuron. 2003;39(2):375–386.

105.

De Groot JH, Semin GR, Smeets MA. I can see, hear, and smell your fear: comparing olfactory and audiovisual media in fear communication. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2014;143(2):825.

106.

Jacobs LF, Arter J, Cook A, Sulloway FJ. Olfactory orientation and navigation in humans. PLoS One. 2015;10(6):e0129387.

107.

Olivetti Belardinelli M, Federici S, Delogu F, Palmiero M. Sonification of spatial information: audio-tactile exploration strategies by normal and blind subjects. In: Stephanidis C. (ed) Universal Access in Human-Computer Interaction. Intelligent and Ubiquitous Interaction Environments. UAHCI 2009. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, vol 5615. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer; 2009.

108.

Richard E, Tijou A, Richard P, Ferrier J-L. Multi-modal virtual environments for education with haptic and olfactory feedback. Virtual Real. 2006;10(3):207–225.

109.

Farserotu J, Babrowski J, Decotignie J-D, et al. Smart skin for tactile prosthetics. Paper presented at: 2012 6th International Symposium on Medical Information and Communication Technology (ISMICT), La Jolla, CA, USA; 2012.

Creative Commons License © 2018 The Author(s). This work is published and licensed by Dove Medical Press Limited. The full terms of this license are available at https://www.dovepress.com/terms.php and incorporate the Creative Commons Attribution - Non Commercial (unported, v3.0) License. By accessing the work you hereby accept the Terms. Non-commercial uses of the work are permitted without any further permission from Dove Medical Press Limited, provided the work is properly attributed. For permission for commercial use of this work, please see paragraphs 4.2 and 5 of our Terms.