Back to Archived Journals » Neurobehavioral HIV Medicine » Volume 7

Evidence-based perspectives on the implementation of screening for neurocognitive impairment in HIV

Authors Haddow L, Barber T, Breuer L, Cartledge J, Pierce K

Received 22 March 2016

Accepted for publication 1 June 2016

Published 4 August 2016 Volume 2016:7 Pages 31—41

DOI https://doi.org/10.2147/NBHIV.S91147

Checked for plagiarism Yes

Review by Single anonymous peer review

Peer reviewer comments 5

Editor who approved publication: Dr Stephen Ferrando



Lewis J Haddow,1 Tristan J Barber,2,3 Louise Breuer,4 Jonathan D Cartledge,4 Katherine Pierce5

1Research Department of Infection and Population Health, University College London, 2St Stephen’s AIDS Trust, Chelsea and Westminster Hospital NHS Foundation Trust, 3Faculty of Medicine, Imperial College London, 4Department of Genitourinary Medicine, 5Camden Neurological and Stroke Service, Central and North West London NHS Foundation Trust, London, UK

Abstract: This review considers the justification, accuracy, limitations, and potential applications of screening instruments for neurocognitive impairment (NCI) in HIV-positive patients. Wilson and Jungner’s classic screening principles provide thinking tools to guide discussion: the condition should be an important health problem; there should be an accepted treatment; there should be a suitable test; and the natural history of the condition should be adequately understood. NCI appears to be common among HIV-positive patients, but evidence of its progression in those established on suppressive antiretroviral therapy is inconclusive. Also, there are limited data on the optimum management in patients who are found to have NCI but are already receiving effective antiretroviral therapy. The accuracy of screening tests, such as the HIV Dementia Scale and the Montreal Cognitive Assessment, is such that their positive and negative likelihood ratios are close to 1; therefore, many false positive and negative results will arise when using these tests in most clinical scenarios. We argue that the place for screening for neurocognitive impairment is within a management pathway that aims to identify a range of psychological and neurological problems. Widespread implementation of screening for HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders is premature, on the basis of available evidence.

Keywords: HIV, cognition disorders, neuropsychological tests, sensitivity and specificity, diagnosis

Neuropsychological health of people living with HIV

HIV is known to establish productive infection of microglia and nonproductive infection of astrocytes,1 causing HIV-associated dementia (HAD) or HIV encephalopathy (HIVE) in some untreated patients.2 Effects include cognitive, motor, and behavioral deficits, often affecting attention and executive functions. Prior to 1995, when it was progressive and fatal in the absence of treatment, HAD/HIVE was reported in observational cohorts at a rate of 6.5 cases per 1,000 patient-years.3 With the increasing availability of antiretroviral therapy (ART), the incidence, prevalence, severity, and character have changed.46 Neurocognitive impairment (NCI) has been reported in 19%–69% of people living with HIV (PLWH) in recent cross-sectional studies from Western Europe and the US, with variations in prevalence depending on the population sampled and the control group.719

There is no diagnostic test for HIV-associated neurocognitive disorder (HAND) from neuroimaging or laboratory investigations.20,21 Diagnostic criteria have evolved over the past 3 decades to reflect the changing pathology and epidemiology of HIV neuropathogenesis, with the Frascati criteria being the most recent.2,17,22 Diagnosis relies on neuropsychological testing, where normative neuropsychological data for the populations of interest are often lacking and diagnostic thresholds are arbitrary.2325 Other pathologies are excluded through neuroimaging and lumbar puncture, but many HIV-positive (HIV+) patients have other psychological morbidities that confound diagnosis.

Many other mental health problems are common in PLWH, with depression perhaps the most prominent.26 Meta-analysis of ten studies comparing depression rates in HIV+ and HIV− negative individuals found approximately double the risk of depression compared with the general population, although the variation between studies was high.27 Generalized and specific anxiety disorders frequently occur in PLWH,2830 and suicidality is also an important concern.31 Linked to these disorders, alcohol use is prevalent in some HIV+ groups, particularly men and drug users.32 Illicit drug use has long been associated with the AIDS epidemic, with injecting drug users being a core risk group. Heavy use of recreational drugs is recognized as being prevalent in HIV+ men who have sex with men, often in the context of higher risk sexual behavior.33 Drugs of abuse, particularly methamphetamine, opiates, and cocaine, have overlapping toxicities with the neuropathogenesis of HIV.34,35 These patterns of mental health may be caused by the stress, stigma, and social difficulties of living with HIV, or they may precede or coassociate with the infection. The overlap between mental disorders and NCI may both increase the risk of HAND and confound its diagnosis.

In future, there may be a higher rate of cognitive decline in PLWH resulting from incidental neurodegenerative disease. As the survival of treated HIV+ populations approaches that of the general population,36 the proportion living to middle and older age increases.37,38 Those in age groups at highest risk of Alzheimer’s disease are still relatively uncommon. HIV’s association with chronic inflammation, other cardiovascular diseases, endothelial dysfunction,39 and ischemic stroke40,41 suggests a higher risk of vascular dementia in PLWH.

Outcomes and impacts of HIV-associated NCI in treated patients

The first of Wilson and Jungner’s criteria42 is the condition sought should be an important health problem. The high prevalence of NCI in cross-sectional studies suggests that it is a common, persistent problem even in treated HIV.7 ­Notwithstanding the uncertainty surrounding its true ­prevalence, NCI is an important concern for patients and clinicians. Patients with good virological control are still at risk of mild-to-moderate impairment, and complications include increased mortality,43 impaired quality of life,44 impaired everyday function,45 and poor adherence to ART.43

It is now apparent that ART can prevent or reverse the disease process, and the specter of HAD has diminished since the 1980s.6 This is supported by population-level cohort data3,4 and individual-level results in which the effects of ART were measured relative to a pretreatment baseline. The effects are reassuring in patients with significant NCI before treatment, with 30%–70% experiencing improved cognitive function in the first few years of ART.6,46

Without wishing to minimize the potential impact of NCI on PLWH, there are also reassuring data suggesting that cognitive function generally remains stable over 1–5 years. Cole et al47 analyzed data from 345 stable HIV+ patients in US Multicenter AIDS Cohort Study with a brief cognitive and psychomotor assessment and found preserved function over 5 years compared to controls. Recent data from the Multicenter AIDS Cohort Study, using a more comprehensive neuropsychological battery, found little change in the prevalence of HAND from 2007 to 2012 and minimal progression over 4 years.48 In a 1-year study in the People’s Republic of China49 and a 3-year study in the US,50 both cohorts including a high proportion of nonvirologically suppressed patients, the proportion of patients who declined in neurocognitive function was similar to the proportion who improved. Perhaps controversially, there was a considerable amount of fluctuation in neurocognitive performance.51 In two 2-year cohorts from Italy and France with 95% ART coverage, the prevalence of NCI was very similar both at baseline and at study completion.52,53 An earlier US cohort study, enrolled between 2001 and 2005, found declining cognitive function in 35% of the 215 patients followed up (of 276 originally enrolled), but there was a 56% prevalence of “substance abuse or dependence”, and treatment and adherence may have been suboptimal by current standards.54

Other classic screening criteria are that the natural history of a condition, including its development from silent to clinical disease, should be well-understood, and there should be a recognizable latent or early symptomatic stage. Beyond the beneficial effects of ART, physicians are faced with uncertainty about how patients’ cognitive difficulties will evolve over time, and more longitudinal studies are needed. There are reports that patients with asymptomatic NCI (ANI)17 experience further decline in cognitive function. In a multicenter US HIV cohort study (n=387), there was a substantially greater rate of change from ANI to more severe grades of HAND than in those without NCI.55 Similar results were found in a Canadian cohort where virological suppression rates were much higher than in the US study, but there was still a 32% risk of progression from ANI to other grades of HAND, compared to 17% risk of progression from normal cognitive function to symptomatic HAND.56 Limitations to both these studies lie in the use of “time to event” analysis, which in this context assumes that once a patient changes from ANI to more severe grades of HAND, they cannot revert to having ANI or normal cognitive function. As a result, the analysis will capture greater variability in function as well as faster decline. Also in the Canadian study, progression was determined from participants’ self-reported abilities, which could have been significantly influenced by mood and other factors.

Treatment options for HAND

Another important prerequisite for screening is that there should be an accepted treatment and there should be an agreed policy on whom to treat.42 As already noted, ART is highly effective in reversing HIVE, and as a result, HIV treatment guidelines have included symptomatic HAND as an indication for ART.57,58 But the Strategic Timing of ­AntiRetroviral Treatment study suggests that initiation of ART is not the issue: we should be treating everyone with HIV, and the results of neuropsychological assessments have no bearing on whether or not a patient should be on ART.59 There is a case to be made for HIV testing in individuals with NCI who are not already diagnosed, but that argument has been strongly made elsewhere.60

If the benefits of ART in treating or preventing HAND or HIVE are uncontroversial, we must consider whether there is a case for screening patients already on ART so that other therapeutic interventions can be offered. Considerations include whether any particular ART drug or regimen affects neurocognitive outcomes in patients with demonstrable NCI, or in PLWH in general; whether additional benefit can be achieved from adjunctive treatments; and whether more general approaches, such as management of comorbidities and psychological and pharmacological therapies can improve outcomes. Regarding the last point, there is a benefit in identifying individuals with functional impairment because of the opportunities to help those individuals in other ways, such as social support and compensatory neurorehabilitation.61 But for those assessed as having ANI, there is no clear path for treatment despite evidence of an increased risk of progression.

The central nervous system (CNS) penetration effectiveness score grades the ability of antiretroviral drugs to suppress viral replication in the CNS by achieving adequate tissue concentrations.62 Similarly, the monocyte efficacy score grades drugs on their ability to achieve high intracellular concentrations in the macrophage-monocyte cell line, which includes microglia.63 To date, there have been inconsistent findings regarding the effects of drugs with different CNS penetrations.46,62,6470 The inevitable biases that emerge in these observational studies make their findings difficult to interpret. In an attempt to overcome these biases, a randomized treatment modification trial compared high- and low-penetration regimens but was terminated early because of under-recruitment.71 Concerns have been raised about the effectiveness of boosted protease inhibitor (PI) monotherapy because of PIs’ relatively poor penetration into the CNS,72 but two trials reported no differences in neurocognitive function between patients randomized to PI monotherapy and combination antiretroviral therapy over at least 96 weeks of follow-up.73,74 While British and European HIV treatment guidelines recommend any standard combination antiretroviral therapy regimen in most patients with NCI, American guidelines name dolutegravir and darunavir as preferred options. There are also potential adverse neuropsychological effects of ART, particularly efavirenz,7577 and efavirenz-containing regimens should be avoided in those with neurocognitive difficulties.

Some patients with NCI have an underlying phenomenon of detectable virus in cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) but undetectable virus in plasma, often known as CSF viral escape or discordance. This has been described in case series by Canestri et al.7881 Given the association between the phenomenon and neurocognitive or neurological syndromes and the theoretical risk of mutation and “spill-over” of resistant strains into the wider circulation, it is generally recommended that attempts are made in such patients to genotype the virus in both the CSF and plasma and adjust the ART regimen accordingly. One could view screening for NCI as a method of ultimately funneling down to identify such individuals.

Trials of adjunctive pharmacological treatment, including minocycline, selegiline, valproate, rivastigmine, and lithium,8286 have not shown benefits in cognitive function in PLWH. It may be that the trials were underpowered or too short in duration to show benefit, or that too heterogeneous a group of patients was targeted. Or, given current gaps in knowledge on neuropathogenesis in patients receiving virologically suppressive ART, it may be that the wrong interventions were chosen.

HIV-specific screening tools for neurocognitive disorders

Screening also requires a suitable test or examination, which should be acceptable to the population.42 The first bespoke screening tool was the HIV Dementia Scale (HDS), published in 1995 and intended as “a brief but sensitive instrument to identify dementia (in HIV+ patients)”.87 The scale comprises four subtests (Table 1), targeted mainly at subcortical cognitive processes. A modified HDS, which omits the antisaccadic errors subtest, has also been proposed.88 Development of the scale involved assessment of 101 HIV+ and 29 seronegative participants in 1991–1992.

Table 1 Subtests of the HIV Dementia Scale and International HIV Dementia Scale

There have been four systematic reviews and three meta-analyses (Table 2) of the accuracy of the HDS.8992 It is clear from the pooled estimates that the sensitivity of the scale is low and the specificity is only slightly better, while differences in methodology and study selection have led to heterogeneous estimates between meta-analyses and individual studies. The HDS is designed to be highly standardized, so heterogeneity is more likely to result from the diverse range of testing batteries, case definitions, and diagnostic thresholds used in the “gold standard”. One can resolve this heterogeneity by reducing sensitivity and specificity into a single diagnostic odds ratio (DOR), at the expense of a considerable amount of useful information. The results of two of the meta-analyses give the DORs of 5.88 and 7.52.89,91

Table 2 Summary of three meta-analyses of the accuracy of the HDS and IHDS


Notes: aPositive likelihood ratio = sensitivity/(1–specificity) and negative likelihood ratio = (1–sensitivity)/specificity. bZipursky et al did not present a pooled specificity rate; therefore, likelihood ratios could not be calculated.


Abbreviations: HAD, HIV-associated dementia; HAND, HIV-associated neurocognitive disorder; HDS, HIV Dementia Scale; IHDS, International HIV Dementia Scale; MND, minor neurocognitive disorder; ND, not done.

The three reviews have highlighted problems with study quality in diagnostic accuracy studies. The first major concern was the possibility of validation bias, which occurs when assessors in a diagnostic accuracy study are not blinded to the results of the other test, ie, they are not blinded to the results of the HDS when carrying out the neuropsychological battery and they are not blinded to the full battery when conducting the HDS. Validation bias can lead to falsely high estimates of accuracy. The second major concern was participant selection, which in almost all studies involved some exclusion of patients with competing psychiatric, neurological, or systemic conditions. By excluding such individuals, diagnostic accuracy studies create an artificially “clean” study sample, leading to an overestimate of diagnostic accuracy and results that may not be generalizable to real clinical scenarios. The use of such entry criteria, in conjunction with nonrandom, nonconsecutive sampling strategies, is likely to have led to overestimation of accuracy.93

It was highlighted in the review by Haddow et al that almost all published studies of the HDS were conducted in North America,89 although the scale has been specifically tested in Spanish speakers.94,95 In a Zambian study, it was observed that all 48 HIV+ participants and 15 presumed seronegative controls tested positively on the HDS.96 In response to difficulties with culturally specific elements of the HDS and the administration of the antisaccadic errors subtest, the International HIV Dementia Scale (IHDS) was developed as a “cross-cultural screening instrument” for resource-limited settings (Table 1).97 The IHDS was originally developed by analyzing two HIV+ patient samples, based in the US (n=66) and Uganda (n=81). Most subsequent studies of its accuracy have been conducted in low- and middle-income countries. It has been modified for use in several different languages.97103

The results of meta-analyses of the IHDS are also summarized in Table 2. The sensitivity is, in general, higher than that of the HDS, but the specificity and the positive likelihood ratio (LR) are poor, and the overall DOR is lower (pooled estimates from 2.56 to 3.49).89,91 The limitations of studies of the IHDS are similar to those affecting studies of the HDS. Furthermore, concerns around cultural bias have not been completely resolved with the IHDS. One study in India reported that scores on the IHDS were statistically associated with education but not with HIV status.104 This highlights the concern that an estimate of premorbid neurocognitive function is as important when using short screening tools as it is for full neuropsychological evaluation.

While most studies report sensitivity and specificity as their main results, these statistics only tell us how the result of the test is likely to turn out, given certain assumptions about the result of the gold standard diagnosis. In clinical practice, one usually wishes to know the likelihood of a patient truly having or not having a condition in the face of a positive or negative test. To derive this, two parameters are needed: the prior probability that the condition is present and either a positive LR (LR+) or a negative LR (LR-), depending on the result of the test (formula given in the footnote of Table 2). According to Jaeschke et al, tests with LR+ >5 or LR- <0.2 provide “strong” evidence for or against a diagnosis, and LR+ >10 and LR- <0.1 are needed to provide “convincing” diagnostic evidence.105

We can consider the situation of a case where the HDS or IHDS is used in a setting of high background prevalence, eg, where one-third of patients screened are considered to be affected by HAND (a probability of 33.3%; odds of “2:1 against” or 0.5), and apply rough estimates of LR+ and LR- derived from the reviews cited earlier. Given a positive HDS, the posttest odds are 1.5 or 3:2 in favor, so the probability of the patient having HAND is 60%. A positive IHDS results in the posttest odds of 0.8 or 5:4 against, a posttest probability of 44%. On the other hand, a negative HDS would lead to posttest odds of 4:1 against or a probability of 20%, and a negative IHDS would give the posttest odds of 0.3 or 10:3 against, a probability of 23%. Table 3 summarizes this scenario and the following two other scenarios: high clinical suspicion where the tests are used diagnostically and prior probability is assumed to be 66.7%, and screening in a lower prevalence setting. The extent to which either the HDS or IHDS advances the diagnostic process in these scenarios is debatable.

Table 3 Calculation of posttest probability of HAND using the HDS and IHDS, under three different fictional scenarios


Note: aAssumptions in the table are that positive likelihood ratios are 3.0 for the HDS and 1.6 for the IHDS and that negative likelihood ratios are 0.5 for the HDS and 0.6 for the IHDS. These assumptions are approximations of the results of published meta-analyses,8991 rounded off for easier arithmetic.


Abbreviations: HAND, HIV-associated neurocognitive disorder; HDS, HIV Dementia Scale; IHDS, International HIV Dementia Scale.

Non-HIV-specific neurocognitive screening tools used to assess HIV+ patients

The mini–mental state Examination (MMSE) is a long-established tool for rapidly assessing cognitive function.106 Although frequently used in acute medicine and geriatric care, the tool has found little application in studies of HAND. An important study, albeit small in sample size (33 HIV+ and 13 seronegative participants), was that of Skinner et al, in which participants were tested with the MMSE, HDS, and IHDS.107 The MMSE showed a sensitivity of 46% and a specificity of 55% at the standard cut-off of <27, and the area under the receiver–operator curve was 0.48. These statistics suggest that the MMSE is little better than chance in its discriminatory power for detecting HAND.

There is an expanding literature on the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) in the assessment of HIV+ patients (Table 4). This tool includes brief tests of construction (cube copy and clock face), picture naming, learning and recall of five words, digit span forward and backward, verbal fluency, abstraction, and attention and orientation in time and place. Although it may take longer to perform than the HDS and IHDS and requires a preprinted score card, it is highly standardized, easy to conduct, and freely available online (www.mocatest.org). The MoCA has a wider dynamic range of scores than the shorter screening tools, and different thresholds have been studied. In a study by Joska et al,108 the MoCA showed higher sensitivity and lower specificity than any of the other tools used in the study (including the HDS, IHDS, MMSE, and Simioni symptom questionnaire [SSQ]). A study in the US found results close to those of Joska,109 while work in the Netherlands showed similar high sensitivity and low specificity only when used at a cut-off of 28.5; at standard cut-off, both statistics were closer to 60%.110 In the Dutch study, the area under the receiver–operator curve suggested that the MoCA was diagnostically similar to the HDS. In contrast, several other studies, including one recruiting only participants aged >60 years and one using a locally modified version in Korea, have also reported lower sensitivity but higher specificity.111113 Thus, there is a wide range of diagnostic accuracy estimates for the MoCA, similar to the HDS and IHDS. In all studies, the LR+ is <5 and the LR– is >0.2, implying that the test is limited in its diagnostic power.

Table 4 Summary of results from studies of diagnostic accuracy of the MoCA at standard cutoff (26 points)


Notes: aRange of sensitivity estimates relates to differing thresholds in the gold standard diagnosis. bPositive likelihood ratio = sensitivity/(1–specificity) and negative likelihood ratio = (1–sensitivity)/specificity.


Abbreviations: DOR, diagnostic odds ratio; LR, likelihood ratio; MoCA, Montreal Cognitive Assessment.

The MoCA is also limited by its cultural specificity, as illustrated by work conducted in a Xhosa-speaking sample of HIV+ patients and seronegative controls in South Africa.114 In that study, the mean score was well below the standard cut-off for impairment in both groups. The study challenged assumptions about precisely which items in a screening tool may not have cross-cultural validity: in a picture-naming task, the rhinoceros, an animal indigenous to South Africa, was frequently misnamed as an elephant, buffalo, or hippopotamus. Also, the language tasks can be particularly difficult to transpose into non-Indo-European languages, such as Xhosa and Korean.113,114

The MoCA has its limitations, but its main advantage over the HDS and IHDS may be that it provides more qualitative information and covers a wider range of cognitive tasks, thus contributing to the “narrative” around a patient’s neurocognitive symptoms and abilities.

Short neuropsychological testing batteries used as screening tools

Another screening approach is the use of focused neuropsychological batteries. The Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status (RBANS) is a popular format in some centers, but there are few published results of its use specifically in HIV+ populations, and it lacks sufficient assessment of executive function. Other short batteries include the neurocognitive HIV study screen,115 the brief neurocognitive screen,29 the NeuroScreen,116 the high sensitivity cognitive screen,117 and multiple other short combinations of standardized tests, which have been reviewed systematically in two publications.90,92 Computerized tools, such as Cogstate and CalCAP, may be used as screening instruments or as more detailed batteries.9,118,119 Despite widespread use in clinical research, computerized assessment is infrequently encountered in routine practice.

There does not appear to be consensus on the best short battery to use in HIV. Most tools published have >70% sensitivity but lack specificity to be able to distinguish HAND from other conditions, and the overall quality of published diagnostic accuracy studies is not high. The main advantages of brief batteries may be their application in multicenter cohorts or trials and their potential role in task-shifting ­assessments away from neuropsychologists, either as a preliminary or as a full assessment. At this level of intensity, assessments occupy a place that is intermediate between screening and diagnostic testing.

Neuropsychological symptom screening in HIV+ patients

Perhaps the simplest approach to screening for HAND is through brief, structured symptom questionnaires. Arguably, screening that targets unrecognized symptomatic disease (not just true asymptomatic disease) is still screening.42 One widely used method is known as the SSQ, after the first author of study in which it was first published,12 although other short questionnaires in use include the four cognitive impairment symptom questions from the Medical Outcomes Study (MOS-HIV).120

Simioni et al used data from 50 Swiss patients who complained of symptoms affecting memory, concentration, or reasoning and 50 who were symptom free, all with undetectable viral loads. There was an unusually high prevalence of HAND (69%) and fairly poor accuracy for the SSQ (sensitivity 57% and specificity 69%).12 In unpublished work from a British and Irish study of HIV+ patients aged >50 years, one of the three questions of the SSQ, concerning memory, showed a statistically significant association with neurocognitive performance measured using Cogstate.121 Other works, such as the Spanish neurocognitive HIV study115,122 and analysis of a German-language version,123 have shown poor accuracy, probably no better than chance. The SSQ entered the European AIDS Clinical Society treatment guidelines in 2011 as a recommended tool for screening for NCI,58 but this has since been changed to a recommendation “to guide physician assessment” in 2015.124 British guidelines recommend the questionnaire as one possible screening tool for the routine identification of psychological support needs.125

The MOS-HIV cognitive impairment questions were initially validated by Knippels et al, who showed an association between questionnaire responses and neuropsychological test performance in most cognitive outcome measures (correlation with overall neuropsychological performance: β coefficient =0.252, P<0.01).126 In a large four-nation study using a shorter computerized assessment, there was no association between the MOS-HIV questions and neuropsychological test performance on Cogstate.127

The relative lack of data on short symptom questionnaires and the volume of ongoing research in this area indicate that conclusions about their appropriate use in screening may be premature. And, it is unlikely that broad symptom questions will be in any way HIV specific, with respect to any other cause of NCI.

Conclusion and perspectives on the practical implementation of screening for HIV-associated NCI

If screening for NCI is indeed necessary in HIV+ patients, we clearly lack the evidence needed to implement it. Since the recommendation to screen passed into national and international guidelines, care providers have been grappling with questions around how to screen: how often, with which tools, and in whom. Furthermore, none of the tools have been assessed for practice effects, which have implications for periodic screening. Reassuringly, according to the limited results available, neurocognitive screening is acceptable to patients.128 However, while earlier European guidelines suggested specific screening intervals of 2 years in most patients and 1 year in higher risk patients,58 a blanket screening approach has been dropped in favor of active case finding.124 One possible strategy that has not been addressed in clinical research is selective screening, limited to patients at high risk of HAND, such as those with a lower nadir CD4 count or other risk factors.129,130

After many years of research, we still lack a suitable screening tool for HAND. The HDS lacks sensitivity, and the IHDS lacks specificity; neither has been tested in a wide enough cultural context. The MoCA gives more detail but is no more accurate than the HDS or IHDS, and available data are similarly heterogeneous. A diverse range of short batteries has been tried, but in general, each battery has only been tested in a single study, and none are significantly better than the shorter tools to justify the additional training and resource requirements. Symptom screening questions appear to be the least accurate and have the least published results of all the approaches, although they have the advantage of being easiest to administer in clinical practice. It may be that the diagnosis is too complex for a bedside tool. It is unlikely that a sufficiently low LR- will be achieved to remove the need for more detailed neuropsychological assessment or brain imaging in patients in whom there is a significant clinical suspicion. And with only a modest LR+, a positive screening test is unlikely to justify intensive investigation in patients with minimal symptoms or clinical suspicion of HAND.

It seems that, both from the available evidence and from clinical experience, symptoms suggestive of NCI are nonspecific. In all likelihood, patients with symptoms affecting memory, concentration, or reasoning may have depression, anxiety, substance misuse, stressful life events, or simply ­normal aging. Screening for these other psychological concerns is a laudable aim, and one should not be dissuaded from using short symptom questionnaires in routine practice. However, widespread use of screening may cause unnecessary anxiety among patients, and too great a focus on HAND may lead to hypervigilance for normal everyday memory lapses in some patients. Furthermore, given the aging of the HIV+ population and the omission of competing diagnoses from many studies of tools’ accuracy, we have little evidence that the HDS, IHDS, and other tests are able to distinguish HAND from other causes of NCI. Screening should be used responsibly and with appropriate counseling, bearing in mind the potential inaccuracy of the tools. Clinicians should not view them as filters solely to identify possible HAND. Instead, a positive result on tools such as the SSQ opens up wider lines of neurological and psychological enquiries, guiding clinicians to explore possible mood disorders, substance use, and so on in more detail.

Used correctly, screening tests are the first step in a clinical management pathway. They provide an opportunity to review patients’ mental health and any concerns that they may have relating to cognitive function. They should be viewed as a part of the assessment of a wide range of psychological concerns and not just as quick or preliminary diagnostic tests for HAND. An evidence-based clinical pathway may be a more useful end goal than further refinements to screening tools. Future studies should go beyond estimates of diagnostic accuracy and test the utility of a multistage assessment pathway in a representative clinical setting.

Disclosure

The authors report no conflicts of interest in this work.

References

1.

Anthony IC, Bell JE. The neuropathology of HIV/AIDS. Int Rev Psychiatry. 2008;20(1):15–24.

2.

Price RW, Brew BJ. The AIDS dementia complex. J Infect Dis. 1988;158(5):1079–1083.

3.

Bhaskaran K, Mussini C, Antinori A, et al; CASCADE Collaboration. Changes in the incidence and predictors of human immunodeficiency virus associated dementia in the era of highly active antiretroviral therapy. Ann Neurol. 2008;63(2):213–221.

4.

Dore GJ, McDonald A, Li Y, Kaldor JM, Brew BJ; National HIV Surveillance Committee. Marked improvement in survival following AIDS dementia complex in the era of highly active antiretroviral therapy. AIDS. 2003;17(10):1539–1545.

5.

Cysique LA, Maruff P, Brew BJ. Prevalence and pattern of neuropsychological impairment in HIV/AIDS infection across pre-HAART and HAART eras: a combined study of 2 cohorts. J Neurovirol. 2004;10(6):350–357.

6.

Joska JA, Gouse H, Paul RH, Stein DJ, Flisher AJ. Does highly active antiretroviral therapy improve neurocognitive function? A systematic review. J Neurovirol. 2010;16(2):101–114.

7.

Tozzi V, Balestra P, Libertone R, Antinori A. Cognitive function in treated HIV patients. Neurobehav HIV Med. 2010;2:95–113.

8.

Maki PM, Rubin LH, Valcour VG, et al. Cognitive function in women with HIV: findings from the Women’s Interagency HIV Study. Neurology. 2015;84(3):231–240.

9.

McDonnell J, Haddow LJ, Daskalopoulou M, et al. Minimal cognitive impairment in UK HIV positive men who have sex with men: effect of case definitions, and comparison with the general population and HIV negative men. J Acquir Immune Defic Syndr. 2014;67(2):120–127.

10.

Crum-Cianflone NF, Moore DJ, Letendre S, et al. Low prevalence of neurocognitive impairment in early diagnosed and managed HIV-infected persons. Neurology. 2013;80(4):371–379.

11.

Heaton RK, Clifford DB, Franklin DRJ, et al; CHARTER Group. HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders persist in the era of potent antiretroviral therapy: CHARTER Study. Neurology. 2010;75(23):2087–2096.

12.

Simioni S, Cavassini M, Annoni JM, et al. Cognitive dysfunction in HIV patients despite long-standing suppression of viremia. AIDS. 2010;24(9):1243–1250.

13.

Bragança M, Palha A. Depression and neurocognitive performance in Portuguese patients infected with HIV. AIDS Behav. 2011;15(8):1879–1887.

14.

Tozzi V, Balestra P, Lorenzini P, et al. Prevalence and risk factors for human immunodeficiency virus-associated neurocognitive impairment, 1996-2002: results from an urban observational cohort. J Neurovirol. 2005;11(3):265–273.

15.

Garvey LJ, Surendrakumar V, Winston A. Low rates of neurocognitive impairment are observed in neuro-asymptomatic HIV-infected subjects on effective antiretroviral therapy. HIV Clin Trials. 2011;12(6):333–338.

16.

Winston A, Arenas-Pinto A, Stoehr W, et al. Neurocognitive function in HIV infected patients on antiretroviral therapy. PLoS One. 2013;8(4):e61949.

17.

Antinori A, Arendt G, Becker JT, et al. Updated research nosology for HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders. Neurology. 2007;69(18):1789–1799.

18.

Robertson KR, Smurzynski M, Parsons TD, et al. The prevalence and incidence of neurocognitive impairment in the HAART era. AIDS. 2007;21(14):1915–1921.

19.

Wright EJ, Grund B, Cysique LA, et al. Factors associated with neurocognitive test performance at baseline: a substudy of the INSIGHT Strategic Timing of AntiRetroviral Treatment (START) trial. HIV Med. 2015;16(suppl 1):97–108.

20.

Ances BM, Hammoud DA. Neuroimaging of HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders (HAND). Curr Opin HIV AIDS. 2014;9(6):545–551.

21.

Brew BJ, Letendre SL. Biomarkers of HIV related central nervous system disease. Int Rev Psychiatry. 2008;20(1):73–88.

22.

Nomenclature and research case definitions for neurologic manifestations of human immunodeficiency virus-type 1 (HIV-1) infection. Report of a Working Group of the American Academy of Neurology AIDS Task Force. Neurology. 1991;41(6):778–785.

23.

Gisslén M, Price RW, Nilsson S. The definition of HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders: are we overestimating the real prevalence? BMC Infect Dis. 2011;11:356.

24.

Meyer AC, Boscardin WJ, Kwasa JK, Price RW. Is it time to rethink how neuropsychological tests are used to diagnose mild forms of HIV-Associated Neurocognitive Disorders? Impact of false-positive rates on prevalence and power. Neuroepidemiology. 2013;41(3–4):208–216.

25.

Arenas-Pinto A, Winston A, Stohr W, et al. Neurocognitive function in HIV-infected patients: comparison of two methods to define impairment. PLoS One. 2014;9(7):e103498.

26.

Ownby RL, Jacobs RJ, Waldrop-Valverde D, Gould F. Depression care and prevalence in HIV-positive individuals. Neurobehav HIV Med. 2010;2:73–83.

27.

Ciesla JA, Roberts JE. Meta-analysis of the relationship between HIV infection and risk for depressive disorders. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158(5):725–730.

28.

Kee MK, Lee SY, Kim NY, et al. Anxiety and depressive symptoms among patients infected with human immunodeficiency virus in South Korea. AIDS Care. 2015;27(9):1174–1182.

29.

Robertson K, Bayon C, Molina JM, et al. Screening for neurocognitive impairment, depression, and anxiety in HIV-infected patients in Western Europe and Canada. AIDS Care. 2014;26(12):1555–1561.

30.

Sewell MC, Goggin KJ, Rabkin JG, Ferrando SJ, McElhiney MC, Evans S. Anxiety syndromes and symptoms among men with AIDS: a longitudinal controlled study. Psychosomatics. 2000;41(4):294–300.

31.

Keiser O, Spoerri A, Brinkhof MW, et al. Suicide in HIV-infected individuals and the general population in Switzerland, 1988-2008. Am J Psychiatry. 2010;167(2):143–150.

32.

Scott-Sheldon LA, Walstrom P, Carey KB, Johnson BT, Carey MP, MASH Research Team. Alcohol use and sexual risk behaviors among individuals infected with HIV: a systematic review and meta-analysis 2012 to early 2013. Curr HIV/AIDS Rep. 2013;10(4):314–323.

33.

Bourne A, Reid D, Hickson F, Torres-Rueda S, Weatherburn P. Illicit drug use in sexual settings (‘chemsex’) and HIV/STI transmission risk behaviour among gay men in South London: findings from a qualitative study. Sex Transm Infect. 2015;91(8):564–568.

34.

Burdo TH, Katner SN, Taffe MA, Fox HS. Neuroimmunity, drugs of abuse, and neuroAIDS. J Neuroimmune Pharmacol. 2006;1(1):41–49.

35.

Silverstein PS, Shah A, Weemhoff J, Kumar S, Singh DP, Kumar A. HIV-1 gp120 and drugs of abuse: interactions in the central nervous system. Curr HIV Res. 2012;10(5):369–383.

36.

Rodger AJ, Lodwick R, Schechter M, et al. Mortality in well controlled HIV in the continuous antiretroviral therapy arms of the SMART and ESPRIT trials compared with the general population. AIDS. 2013;27(6):973–979.

37.

Skingsley A, Kirwan P, Yin Z, et al. HIV New Diagnoses, Treatment and Care in the UK 2015 Report: Data to End 2014. London: Public Health England; 2015.

38.

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. HIV Surveillance Report, 2014. Atlanta, GA: Centers for Disease Control and Prevention; 2015.

39.

Islam FM, Wu J, Jansson J, Wilson DP. Relative risk of cardiovascular disease among people living with HIV: a systematic review and meta-analysis. HIV Med. 2012;13(8):453–468.

40.

Sen S, Rabinstein AA, Elkind MSV, Powers WJ. Recent developments regarding human immunodeficiency virus infection and stroke. Cerebrovasc Dis. 2012;33:209–218.

41.

Benjamin LA, Corbett EL, Connor MD, et al. HIV, antiretroviral treatment, hypertension, and stroke in Malawian adults: a case-control study. Neurology. 2016;86(4):324–333.

42.

Wilson JMG, Jungner G. Principles and Practice of Screening for Disease. Geneva: World Health Organization; 1968.

43.

Tozzi V, Balestra P, Serraino D, et al. Neurocognitive impairment and survival in a cohort of HIV-infected patients treated with HAART. AIDS Res Hum Retroviruses. 2005;21(8):706–713.

44.

Heaton RK, Marcotte TD, Mindt MR, et al. The impact of HIV-associated neuropsychological impairment on everyday functioning. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2004;10(3):317–331.

45.

Woods SP, Iudicello JE, Moran LM, Carey CL, Dawson MS, Grant I. HIV-associated prospective memory impairment increases risk of dependence in everyday functioning. Neuropsychology. 2008;22(1):110–117.

46.

Cysique LA, Waters EK, Brew BJ. Central nervous system antiretroviral efficacy in HIV infection: a qualitative and quantitative review and implications for future research. BMC Neurol. 2011;11:148.

47.

Cole MA, Margolick JB, Cox C, et al. Longitudinally preserved psychomotor performance in long-term asymptomatic HIV-infected individuals. Neurology. 2007;69(24):2213–2220.

48.

Sacktor N, Skolasky RL, Seaberg E, et al. Prevalence of HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders in the Multicenter AIDS Cohort Study. Neurology. 2016;86(4):334–340.

49.

Cysique LA, Letendre SL, Ake C, et al. Incidence and nature of cognitive decline over 1 year among HIV-infected former plasma donors in China. AIDS. 2010;24(7):983–990.

50.

Heaton RK, Franklin DR, Deutsch R, et al. Neurocognitive change in the era of HIV combination antiretroviral therapy: the longitudinal CHARTER study. Clin Infect Dis. 2015;60(3):473–480.

51.

Haddow LJ, Pierce KJ, Daskalopoulou M, Lampe F, Rodger AJ. Neurocognitive change observed in the CHARTER HIV cohort could be due to chance, and may be a cause as well as a consequence of detectable viremia. Clin Infect Dis. 2015;60(9):1441–1442.

52.

Ciccarelli N, Grima P, Fabbiani M, et al. Baseline CD4(+) T-cell count and cardiovascular risk factors predict the evolution of cognitive performance during 2-year follow-up in HIV-infected patients. Antivir Ther. 2015;20(4):433–440.

53.

Dufouil C, Richert L, Thiebaut R, et al. Diabetes and cognitive decline in a French cohort of patients infected with HIV-1. Neurology. 2015;85(12):1065–1073.

54.

Becker BW, Thames AD, Woo E, Castellon SA, Hinkin CH. Longitudinal change in cognitive function and medication adherence in HIV-infected adults. AIDS Behav. 2011;15(8):1888–1894.

55.

Grant I, Franklin DR Jr, Deutsch R, et al. Asymptomatic HIV-associated neurocognitive impairment increases risk for symptomatic decline. Neurology. 2014;82(23):2055–2062.

56.

Rourke SB, Gill J, Rachlis A, et al. Asymptomatic neurocognitive impairment (ANI) is associated with progression to symptomatic HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders (HAND) in people with HIV: results from The Ontario HIV Treatment Network (OHTN) cohort study. 8th IAS Conference on HIV Pathogenesis, Treatment & Prevention. Vancouver, BC: 2015.

57.

Williams IG, Churchill D, Anderson J, et al. British HIV Association guidelines for the treatment of HIV-1-positive adults with antiretroviral therapy. HIV Med. 2012;13(suppl 2):1–85.

58.

Reiss P, Battegay M, Clumeck N, et al. European AIDS Clinical Society Guidelines; Version 6 - October 2011. Paris: European AIDS Clinical Society; 2011.

59.

Lundgren JD, Babiker AG, Gordin F, et al. Initiation of antiretroviral therapy in early asymptomatic HIV infection. N Engl J Med. 2015;373(9):795–807.

60.

Nightingale S, Michael BD, Defres S, Benjamin LA, Solomon T. Test them all; an easily diagnosed and readily treatable cause of dementia with life-threatening consequences if missed. Pract Neurol. 2013;13(6):354–356.

61.

Weber E, Blackstone K, Woods SP. Cognitive neurorehabilitation of HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders: a qualitative review and call to action. Neuropsychol Rev. 2013;23(1):81–98.

62.

Letendre S, Marquie-Beck J, Capparelli E, et al. Validation of the CNS Penetration-Effectiveness rank for quantifying antiretroviral penetration into the central nervous system. Arch Neurol. 2008;65(1):65–70.

63.

Shikuma CM, Nakamoto B, Shiramizu B, et al. Antiretroviral monocyte efficacy score linked to cognitive impairment in HIV. Antivir Ther. 2012;17(7):1233–1242.

64.

Winston A, Duncombe C, Li PCK, et al. Does choice of combination antiretroviral therapy (cART) alter changes in cerebral function testing after 48 weeks in treatment-naive, HIV-1-infected individuals commencing cART? A randomized, controlled study. Clin Infect Dis. 2010;50(6):920–929.

65.

Garvey LJ, Winston A, Walsh J, et al. HIV-associated central nervous system diseases in the recent combination antiretroviral therapy era. Eur J Neurol. 2011;18(3):527–534.

66.

Tozzi V, Balestra P, Salvatori MF, et al. Changes in cognition during antiretroviral therapy: comparison of 2 different ranking systems to measure antiretroviral drug efficacy on HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders. J Acquir Immune Defic Syndr. 2009;52:56–63.

67.

Carvalhal A, Gill MJ, Letendre SL, et al. Central nervous system penetration effectiveness of antiretroviral drugs and neuropsychological impairment in the Ontario HIV Treatment Network Cohort Study. J Neurovirol. 2016;22(3):349–357.

68.

Baker LM, Paul RH, Heaps-Woodruff JM, et al. The effect of central nervous system penetration effectiveness of highly active antiretroviral therapy on neuropsychological performance and neuroimaging in HIV infected individuals. J Neuroimmune Pharmacol. 2015;10(3):487–492.

69.

Fabbiani M, Grima P, Milanini B, et al. Antiretroviral neuropenetration scores better correlate with cognitive performance of HIV-infected patients after accounting for drug susceptibility. Antivir Ther. 2015;20(4):441–447.

70.

Caniglia EC, Cain LE, Justice A, et al. Antiretroviral penetration into the CNS and incidence of AIDS-defining neurologic conditions. Neurology. 2014;83(2):134–141.

71.

Ellis RJ, Letendre SL, Vaida F, et al. Randomized trial of central nervous system-targeted antiretrovirals for HIV-associated neurocognitive disorder. Clin Infect Dis. 2014;58(7):1015–1022.

72.

Perez-Valero I, Bayon C, Cambron I, Gonzalez A, Arribas JR. Protease inhibitor monotherapy and the CNS: peace of mind? J Antimicrob Chemother. 2011;66(9):1954–1962.

73.

Paton NI, Stöhr W, Arenas-Pinto A, et al. Protease inhibitor monotherapy for long-term management of HIV infection: a randomised, controlled, open-label, non-inferiority trial. Lancet HIV. 2015;2(10):e417–e426.

74.

Antinori A, Clarke A, Svedhem-Johansson V, et al. Week 48 efficacy and central nervous system analysis of darunavir/ritonavir monotherapy versus darunavir/ritonavir with two nucleoside analogues. AIDS. 2015;29(14):1811–1820.

75.

Winston A, Puls R, Kerr SJ, et al. Dynamics of cognitive change in HIV-infected individuals commencing three different initial antiretroviral regimens: a randomized, controlled study. HIV Med. 2012;13(4):245–251.

76.

Gazzard BG, Balkin A, Hill A. Analysis of neuropsychiatric adverse events during clinical trials of efavirenz in antiretroviral-naive patients: a systematic review. AIDS Rev. 2010;12(2):67–75.

77.

Pedrol E, Llibre JM, Tasias M, et al. Outcome of neuropsychiatric symptoms related to an antiretroviral drug following its substitution by nevirapine: the RELAX study. HIV Med. 2015;16(10):628–634.

78.

Canestri A, Lescure FX, Jaureguiberry S, et al. Discordance between cerebral spinal fluid and plasma HIV replication in patients with neurological symptoms who are receiving suppressive antiretroviral therapy. Clin Infect Dis. 2010;50(5):773–778.

79.

Edén A, Fuchs D, Hagberg L, et al. HIV-1 viral escape in cerebrospinal fluid of subjects on suppressive antiretroviral treatment. J Infect Dis. 2010;202(12):1819–1825.

80.

Peluso MJ, Ferretti F, Peterson J, et al. Cerebrospinal fluid HIV escape associated with progressive neurologic dysfunction in patients on antiretroviral therapy with well controlled plasma viral load. AIDS. 2012;26(14):1765–1774.

81.

Rawson T, Muir D, Mackie NE, Garvey LJ, Everitt A, Winston A. Factors associated with cerebrospinal fluid HIV RNA in HIV infected subjects undergoing lumbar puncture examination in a clinical setting. J Infect. 2012;65(3):239–245.

82.

Sacktor N, Miyahara S, Deng L, et al. Minocycline treatment for HIV-associated cognitive impairment: results from a randomized trial. Neurology. 2011;77(12):1135–1142.

83.

Schifitto G, Zhong J, Gill D, et al. Lithium therapy for human immunodeficiency virus type 1-associated neurocognitive impairment. J Neurovirol. 2009;15(2):176–186.

84.

Schifitto G, Zhang J, Evans SR, et al. A multicenter trial of selegiline transdermal system for HIV-associated cognitive impairment. Neurology. 2007;69(13):1314–1321.

85.

Simioni S, Cavassini M, Annoni JM, et al. Rivastigmine for HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders: a randomized crossover pilot study. Neurology. 2013;80(6):553–560.

86.

Schifitto G, Peterson DR, Zhong J, et al. Valproic acid adjunctive therapy for HIV-associated cognitive impairment: a first report. Neurology. 2006;66(6):919–921.

87.

Power C, Selnes OA, Grim JA, McArthur JC. HIV dementia scale: a rapid screening test. J Acquir Immune Defic Syndr. 1995;8(3):273–278.

88.

Davis HF, Skolasky RL Jr, Selnes OA, Burgess DM, McArthur JC. Assessing HIV-associated dementia: modified HIV dementia scale versus the Grooved Pegboard. AIDS Read. 2002;12(1):29–31.

89.

Haddow LJ, Floyd S, Copas A, Gilson RJ. A systematic review of the diagnostic accuracy of the HIV dementia scale and International HIV Dementia Scale. PLoS One. 2013;8(4):e61826.

90.

Zipursky AR, Gogolishvili D, Rueda S, et al. Evaluation of brief screening tools for neurocognitive impairment in HIV/AIDS: a systematic review of the literature. AIDS. 2013;27(15):2385–2401.

91.

Hu X, Zhou Y, Long J, et al. Diagnostic accuracy of the International HIV Dementia Scale and HIV dementia scale: a meta-analysis. Exp Ther Med. 2012;4(4):665–668.

92.

Kamminga J, Cysique LA, Lu G, Batchelor J, Brew BJ. Validity of cognitive screens for HIV-associated neurocognitive disorder: a systematic review and an informed screen selection guide. Curr HIV/AIDS Rep. 2013;10(4):342–355.

93.

Lijmer JG, Mol BW, Heisterkamp S, et al. Empirical evidence of design-related bias in studies of diagnostic tests. JAMA. 1999;282:1061–1066.

94.

Levine AJ, Palomo M, Hinkin CH, et al. A comparison of screening batteries in the detection of neurocognitive impairment in HIV-infected Spanish speakers. Neurobehav HIV Med. 2011;3:79–86.

95.

Wojna V, Skolasky RL, McArthur JC, et al. Spanish validation of the HIV dementia scale in women. AIDS Patient Care STDS. 2007;21(12):930–941.

96.

Kvalsund MP, Haworth A, Murman DL, Velie E, Birbeck GL. Closing gaps in antiretroviral therapy access: human immunodeficiency virus-associated dementia screening instruments for non-physician healthcare workers. Am J Trop Med Hyg. 2009;80(6):1054–1059.

97.

Sacktor NC, Wong M, Nakasujja N, et al. The International HIV Dementia Scale: a new rapid screening test for HIV dementia. AIDS. 2005;19:1367–1374.

98.

Nakasujja N, Skolasky RL, Musisi S, et al. Depression symptoms and cognitive function among individuals with advanced HIV infection initiating HAART in Uganda. BMC Psychiatry. 2010;10:44.

99.

Joska JA, Westgarth-Taylor J, Hoare J, et al. Validity of the International HIV Dementia Scale in South Africa. AIDS Patient Care STDS. 2011;25(2):95–101.

100.

Singh D, Sunpath H, John S, Eastham L, Gouden R. The utility of a rapid screening tool for depression and HIV dementia amongst patients with low CD4 counts – a preliminary report. Afr J Psychiatry (Johannesbg). 2008;11(4):282–286.

101.

Choi Y, Townend J, Vincent T, Zaidi I, Sarge-Njie R, Jaye A. Neurologic manifestations of human immunodeficiency virus-2: dementia, myelopathy, and neuropathy in West Africa. J Neurovirol. 2011;17(2):166–175.

102.

Clifford DB, Mitike MT, Mekonnen Y, et al. Neurological evaluation of untreated human immunodeficiency virus infected adults in Ethiopia. J Neurovirol. 2007;13(1):67–72.

103.

Birbeck GL, Kvalsund MP, Byers PA, et al. Neuropsychiatric and socioeconomic status impact antiretroviral adherence and mortality in rural Zambia. Am J Trop Med Hyg. 2011;85(4):782–789.

104.

Waldrop-Valverde D, Nehra R, Sharma S, et al. Education effects on the International HIV Dementia Scale. J Neurovirol. 2010;16(4):264–267.

105.

Jaeschke R, Guyatt GH, Sackett DL. Users’ guides to the medical literature. VI. How to use an article about a diagnostic test. B: What are the results and will they help me in caring for my patients? JAMA. 1994;271:703–707.

106.

Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12(3):189–198.

107.

Skinner S, Adewale AJ, DeBlock L, Gill MJ, Power C. Neurocognitive screening tools in HIV/AIDS: comparative performance among patients exposed to antiretroviral therapy. HIV Med. 2009;10(4):246–252.

108.

Joska JA, Witten J, Thomas KG, et al. A comparison of five brief screening tools for HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders in the USA and South Africa. AIDS Behav. 2016;20. Epub 2016 Feb 9.

109.

Hasbun R, Eraso J, Ramireddy S, et al. Screening for neurocognitive impairment in HIV individuals: the utility of the Montreal Cognitive Assessment test. J AIDS Clin Res. 2012;3(10):186.

110.

Janssen MA, Bosch M, Koopmans PP, Kessels RP. Validity of the Montreal Cognitive Assessment and the HIV dementia scale in the assessment of cognitive impairment in HIV-1 infected patients. J Neurovirol. 2015;21(4):383–390.

111.

Overton ET, Azad T, Parker N, et al. The Alzheimer disease-8 and Montreal Cognitive Assessment as screening tools for neurocognitive impairment in HIV-infected persons. J Neurovirol. 2013;19(1):109–116.

112.

Milanini B, Wendelken LA, Esmaeili-Firidouni P, Chartier M, Crouch P-C, Valcour V. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) to screen for cognitive impairment in HIV over age 60. J Acquir Immune Defic Syndr. 2014;67(1):67–70.

113.

Ku NS, Lee Y, Ahn JY, et al. HIV-associated neurocognitive disorder in HIV-infected Koreans: Korean NeuroAIDS Project. HIV Med. 2014;15(8):470–477.

114.

Robbins RN, Joska JA, Thomas KGF, et al. Exploring the Utility of the Montreal Cognitive Assessment to detect HIV-associated neurocognitive disorder: the challenge and need for culturally valid screening tests in South Africa. Clin Neuropsychol. 2013;27(3):437–454.

115.

Muñoz-Moreno JA, Prats A, Pérez-Álvarez N, et al. A brief and feasible paper-based method to screen for neurocognitive impairment in HIV-infected patients: the NEU screen. J Acquir Immune Defic Syndr. 2013;63(5):585–592.

116.

Ellis RJ, Evans SR, Clifford DB, et al. Clinical validation of the NeuroScreen. J Neurovirol. 2005;11(6):503–511.

117.

Fogel BS. The high sensitivity cognitive screen. Int Psychogeriatr. 1991;3(2):273–288.

118.

Cysique LAJ, Maruff P, Darby D, Brew BJ. The assessment of cognitive function in advanced HIV-1 infection and AIDS dementia complex using a new computerised cognitive test battery. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2006;21:185–194.

119.

Gonzalez R, Heaton RK, Moore DJ, et al. Computerized reaction time battery versus a traditional neuropsychological battery: detecting HIV-related impairments. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2003;9(1):64–71.

120.

Wu AW, Rubin HR, Mathews WC, et al. A health status questionnaire using 30 items from the Medical Outcomes Study. Preliminary validation in persons with early HIV infection. Med Care. 1991;29(8):786–798.

121.

De Francesco D, Underwood J, Post FA, et al. Levels of agreement between commonly used definitions of cognitive impairment and the EACS screening questions in older HIV-positive subjects and matched controls. 15th European AIDS Conference; 21-24 October, 2015. Barcelona: 2015.

122.

Muñoz-Moreno JA, Prats A, Nieto-Verdugo I, et al. Distinct detection of HIV-associated neurocognitive dysfunction according to clinician and patient perception: findings from the NEU study. 6th IAS Conference on HIV Pathogenesis, Treatment and Prevention; 17-20 July 2011. Rome: 2011.

123.

Fasel D, Kunze U, Elzi L, et al. A short tool to screen HIV-infected patients for mild neurocognitive disorders – a pilot study. BMC Psychol. 2014;2(1):21.

124.

Lundgren JD, Gatell JM, Rockstroh JK, Furrer H. European AIDS Clinical Society Guidelines. Huntingdon: EACS; 2015.

125.

MedFASH. British Psychological Society, British HIV Association & Medical Foundation for AIDS & Sexual Health. Standards for Psychological Support for Adults Living with HIV. London: MedFASH; 2011.

126.

Knippels HM, Goodkin K, Weiss JJ, Wilkie FL, Antoni MH. The importance of cognitive self-report in early HIV-1 infection: validation of a cognitive functional status subscale. AIDS. 2002;16(2):259–267.

127.

Haddow LJ, Daskalopoulou M, McDonnell J, et al. Neuropsychological performance and self-reported function in HIV positive patients in five European clinics. 15th European AIDS Conference; 21-24 October, 2015. Barcelona: 2015.

128.

Nightingale A, Ratcliffe D, Leonidou L, et al. HIV-related neurocognitive impairment screening: the patient’s perspective on its utility and psychological impact. AIDS Care. 2014;26(8):1036–1041.

129.

Ellis RJ, Badiee J, Vaida F, et al. Nadir CD4 is a predictor of HIV neurocognitive impairment in the era of combination antiretroviral therapy. AIDS. 2011;25(14):1747–1751.

130.

Cysique LA, Murray JM, Dunbar M, Jeyakumar V, Brew BJ. A screening algorithm for HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders. HIV Med. 2010;11(10):642–649.

Creative Commons License © 2016 The Author(s). This work is published and licensed by Dove Medical Press Limited. The full terms of this license are available at https://www.dovepress.com/terms.php and incorporate the Creative Commons Attribution - Non Commercial (unported, v3.0) License. By accessing the work you hereby accept the Terms. Non-commercial uses of the work are permitted without any further permission from Dove Medical Press Limited, provided the work is properly attributed. For permission for commercial use of this work, please see paragraphs 4.2 and 5 of our Terms.