Back to Journals » Psychology Research and Behavior Management » Volume 16

Does the Power of Social Example Fade? Nudge Effect of Social Information on Individual’s Donation Behaviors During the COVID-19 Pandemic: A Moderated Mediation Model with Three-Wave Cross-Sectional Data

Authors Li S , Li K, Li J

Received 14 December 2022

Accepted for publication 14 March 2023

Published 24 March 2023 Volume 2023:16 Pages 971—987

DOI https://doi.org/10.2147/PRBM.S401420

Checked for plagiarism Yes

Review by Single anonymous peer review

Peer reviewer comments 5

Editor who approved publication: Dr Igor Elman



Shuaiqi Li,1 Kehan Li,2 Jianbiao Li3

1School of Finance, Shandong University of Finance and Economics, Jinan, People’s Republic of China; 2School of Economics, Shandong University of Finance and Economics, Jinan, People’s Republic of China; 3Institute for Study of Brain-Like Economics/School of Economics, Shandong University, Jinan, People’s Republic of China

Correspondence: Kehan Li; Jianbiao Li, Email [email protected]; [email protected]

Purpose: This study assesses how various social information influence individuals’ money donation behaviors towards charitable funds against the COVID-19 pandemic at different stages of the pandemic. It also explores the mediating role of social anxiety and the moderating role of self-control.
Materials and Methods: This three-wave study was conducted with online survey experiments using convenience sampling at the pandemic’s outbreak stage (April–June 2020), trough stage (February–March 2021), and resurgence stage (May 2022) in China. The nudge power of social information was measured by whether participants changed their initial money donation decisions after informed positive or negative social information. Self-report scales were used to measure levels of social anxiety (Social Interaction Anxiety Scale) and self-control (Self-Control Scale). The final data set included 1371 participants from 26 provinces of mainland China. Stata medeff package and SPSS PROCESS were used to analyze the data.
Results: Individuals’ initial donation behaviors did not fluctuate along with the pandemic status, but the nudge effect of social information did. From outbreak stage to trough stage, the nudge power of positive social information significantly declined, but did not significantly change again at the resurgence stage. By contrast, the nudge power of negative social information did not significantly differ between outbreak and trough stage but did significantly increase at the resurgence stage. Social anxiety played a significant mediating role in the relationship between COVID-19 status and power of social information. Moreover, self-control moderated the direct effect of COVID-19 status on power of social information and the indirect effect via social anxiety.
Conclusion: Our findings enrich research on the nudge power variation of social information on individuals’ donation behaviors along with the pandemic status and its potential psychological influence factors. This study also helps guide organizations to better design and carry out social information nudge mechanism.

Keywords: COVID-19, nudge, social information, donation, social anxiety, self-control

Introduction

Since late 2019, the COVID-19 pandemic has devastated lives and livelihoods all over the world.1 The high infection rate of COVID-19 created a new fear in society. Governments worldwide were compelled to implement containment measures in many spheres of life, causing many industries to experience unprecedented economic crises and inevitably exacerbating socio-economic inequalities.2,3 Because COVID-19 has persisted for so long, people have had to rebuild economic and social order while still dealing with the pandemic. This requires solidarity and cooperation, with crucial roles for fairness, generosity, and other kinds of prosociality.

Charitable donation is among the economic activities that best reflect individuals’ prosociality. Amid the pandemic, donations can provide logistical support for the government, health centers, and individuals affected by COVID-19.4 More specifically, pandemic prevention departments and medical units can use donations to implement better prevention and treatment measures. Thus, charitable donations and assistance can strengthen cooperation between government and citizens toward combating COVID-19.

During the unprecedented quarantine of the epidemic, online donation platforms have become one of the most effective ways to make charitable donations.5–7 Online donation platforms often use social example, a kind of social information, as a nudge mechanism to promote citizens’ donation behaviors.8 Few studies have explored whether and how the nudge power of social information changed during the COVID-19 pandemic. Previous studies indicate that the same social information shows different power in different contexts.9,10 Though the outbreak was effectively controlled in mainland China during 2020 to 2021, the rise of the gamma, delta, and other variants have brought a COVID-19 resurgence in 2022. Further, the COVID-19 pandemic has severely impacted the social context in which the social information works. Thus, the different stages of the COVID-19 pandemic may cause changes in the nudge power of social information on individuals’ donation behaviors.

Social Information and Charitable Donation Behavior

Most of previous studies show that social information regarding others’ donations can effectively change an individual’s donation behavior.11–16 However, some studies contend that social information has no impact on the amount of individual donations,17 or even has a negative impact.18,19 The source of social information has been identified as a crucial determinant of its nudge power.12,20,21 In particular, Tian and Konrath22 show that social information has a larger influence on receivers with similar attributes to the information source, which is called the “peer effect”.13 Another main determinant of the power of social information is its content.21 Studies have found that social information conveying appropriate donation behaviors by others has the most positive effect on an individual’s donation decision.11 Social information conveying an excessive or low donation amount has been found to have lower nudge power or even lead to donors making smaller donations.12,13,15

The above studies mainly concentrated on showing donors positive social information about others’ donations. However, negative social information also shows a powerful influence. For example, Dimant23 found that anti-social behavior significantly triggers behavioral contagion as well as pro-social behavior. During the COVID-19 pandemic, citizens have been even more sensitive to negative than to positive social information.24 Thus, it is important to explore the effects of both positive and negative social information.

Social Context and the Nudge Power of Social Information

Previous studies show that social contextual factors may influence the nudge power of social information.10 Thus, the effects of the social information can vary across time and context.25 For example, in their study of the factors influencing consumers’ prosocial behaviors, Goldstein et al26 found that the message “The majority (75%) of guests reuse their towels” increased reuse. However, providing hotel guests with the same social information in Germany did not change their prosocial behaviors.27 These conflicting results derived from the general difference in environment-related attitudes and behaviors between the two countries. Thus, the social information of 75% simply is not high enough for the Germans with high environmental awareness to change their behaviors which is different from the Americans.26 These studies indicate that identical social information may take on different meanings for people in different cultures or social contexts,28 resulting in different effects on individual behavior.9

The COVID-19 pandemic has severely impacted the social context, with varying impacts on people’s mental states, beliefs, or behavior motivations at different stages of the crisis.29 Amid such changes in the social context, individual perceptions of identical social information may have also changed. Thus, the nudge power of identical social information may differ in line with the severity of the disasters. For instance, Wang et al30 found that the nudging effect of blood users’ demand information was enhanced as the COVID-19 pandemic processes to its peak. However, to our knowledge, no study has explored how the nudge power of identical social information on individuals’ donation behaviors varied with the status of the COVID-19.

Social Anxiety as a Mediator

Individual processing of social information is affected by contextual factors such as emotion.31–33 Studies have found that citizens’ emotions changed according to the status of the COVID-19 pandemic,34,35 resulting in different perceptions of identical social information. While quarantine is considered especially effective for reducing infections and mortality, and has been implemented in most countries during the pandemic,36,37 this policy diminishes social opportunities and aggravates loneliness and feelings of uncertainty, leading to higher levels of social anxiety.38,39 With elevated social anxiety, an individual’s information-processing ability tends to be distorted.40,41 Individuals with high social anxiety expect to be more negatively evaluated by others and are more fearful of negative evaluations compared to individuals with low social anxiety. Consequently, they tend to be more influenced by social information.35 Therefore, people with different levels of social anxiety will process the same social information differently. This suggests that with changes over time in the severity of the pandemic, and thus of the quarantine policy, resulting changes in the levels of social anxiety may have altered the power of social information. For instance, as individuals’ social anxiety reduced when the pandemic abated, the nudge power of social information may have declined.

Self-Control as a Moderator

It is speculated in this study that self-control may significantly moderate the relationship between the COVID-19 pandemic stage and the nudge power of social information. Self-control is the ability to pursue overarching goals despite short-term temptations, distractions, or aversive states.42–44 When facing external environment changes, individuals with higher self-control ability are more likely to suppress the influence of those changes on their perceptions.45,46 The weaker one’s self-control, the greater the likelihood of being influenced by external factors without thinking.47 As discussed above, individuals’ perceptions of identical social information may differ according to the social context, in turn affecting their donation decision. Therefore, for individuals with high self-control ability, changes in the status of the COVID-19 pandemic likely have less influence on the power of social information.

Besides, self-control may also moderate the relationship between COVID-19 pandemic status and individuals’ social anxiety levels. Previous studies suggest that self-control is associated with greater emotional stability48 and that emotional stability can even be increased after self-control training.49 Thus, when facing changes in the COVID-19 pandemic status, individuals with high self-control ability are more likely to tune out the effects of environmental change and to maintain emotional stability, meaning that their social anxiety levels are less likely to be affected.

The Present Study

This study employs three-wave cross-sectional surveys to examine changes in individuals’ donation behaviors and the power of social information to nudge individuals’ donation behaviors during the COVID-19 pandemic. This study also proposes a moderated mediation model with social anxiety as a potential mediator and self-control as a potential moderator (Figure 1). Based on the reviewed literature, we hypothesize that (1) the severity of the COVID-19 pandemic status can significantly influence both the power of positive and negative social information on individuals’ donation behaviors; (2) that relationship is mediated by social anxiety; and (3) self-control moderates the effect of COVID-19 status on power of social information and the indirect effect via social anxiety.

Figure 1 Hypothesized moderated mediation model.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Three samples of Chinese participants were recruited during April–June 2020 (T1, the outbreak stage of the COVID-19 pandemic in China), February–March 2021 (T2, the trough stage), and May 2022 (T3, the resurgence stage). A total of 1450 participants were initially recruited via the online crowdsourcing platform Sojump (www.wjx.cn) in mainland China by use of its random questionnaire delivery service. The platform Sojump provides functions equivalent to Amazon Mechanical Turk. We excluded 79 participants who did not meet the following criteria: aged 18 years or older, able to understand Chinese, and understood the purpose and process of the study and agreed to participate. The final data set thus included 1371 participants from 26 provinces of mainland China. The sizes of the three samples were n = 700, n = 322, and n = 349. Our study was approved by the Ethics Committees of Shandong University and Shandong University of Finance and Economics (SDU-SE-IRB 2020-010).

Measures

Demographics

Background questions covered participants’ gender, age, number of siblings, average monthly household income, average monthly personal expenditure, and political affiliation.

We used a dummy variable Gender (0 = male, 1 = female) to represent participants’ gender. Participants’ age (18–20, 21–30, 31–40, >40) was represented by the variable Age. The number of siblings (0, 1, 2, ≥3) was represented by the variable Sibling. Political affiliation was represented by the dummy variable Polity, coded 1 if the participant was a member of the Communist Party, and 0 otherwise. The average monthly income (in RMB) of the participant’s household (0–3000, 3001–6000, 6001–9000, >9000) was represented by the variable Fam-income. Finally, the average monthly personal expenditure (in RMB) of the participant (0–1000, 1001–2000, 2001–3000, >3000) was represented by the variable Ind-expenditure.

Measurement of the Nudge Power of Social Information on Participant’s Donation Behaviors

We employed an online experiment to measure the nudge power of social information. The experiment was designed as a two-stage give-or-take donation game, adapted from Dimant.30 We conducted the same experiment with the three respective samples in April–June 2020, February–March 2021, and May 2022.

Before the experiment began, all participants gave written informed consent to participate. The research background, study purpose, response anonymity, and experiment procedure were also explained to participants. Participants were told that they would make a series of decisions on charitable donations to the China Charity Federation’s fund “Against COVID-19, The China Charity Federation is on the Move”. For simplicity, our study was designed for a single charitable foundation. We also informed participants that we will donate real money (RMB) to the China Charity Federation’s fund, the amount depends on the decisions of 10 participants chosen at random. The 10 chosen participants are also to be paid real money (RMB) based on their behaviors in the experiment. That is, if a participant is chosen randomly, her/his decisions would be implemented and would count toward the charity’s funds (ie, that decision would influence the payoffs). The experimenter would then double the value of all Experimental Currency Units (ECUs) remaining in her/his charitable account at the end of the experiment and convert them into RMB (at an exchange rate of ECU 10 = RMB 1) for donation to the charitable fund. Meanwhile, this participant would receive RMB 3 plus the money converted from ECUs in their private account (using the above exchange rate). All other participants who are not selected would receive RMB 3.

At the beginning of the experiment, each participant was assigned two accounts, one private and one charitable, each containing ECU 100. Participants could choose to transfer money from the charitable account to the private account, or from the private account to the charitable account, or to keep the original sums in both accounts.

After a participant made their initial decision, they were informed of the decisions made by two other participants with similar demographic information to their own. They then had the opportunity to modify their initial decision, having been exposed to the social information. Here we design participants to receive information only from their peers to avoid the influence of different information sources.

We measured the nudge power of social information using the variable Power, coded 1 if the participant modified their donation decision, and 0 otherwise.

Measurement of the Type of Social Information

During the experiment, participants were randomly assigned to one of three experimental conditions, categorized according to peers’ donation behavior as reported in the social information. The three conditions were Baseline, Positive, and Negative. In the Baseline condition, participants were informed that two peer participants had chosen to keep both accounts unchanged. In the Positive condition, participants were informed that two peer participants transferred ECU 81 and ECU 78, respectively, from their private accounts to their charitable accounts. In the Negative condition, participants were informed that two peer participants transferred ECU 80 and ECU 79, respectively, from their charitable accounts to their private accounts.

We created the variable Type to represent the type of social information. Type was coded 0 for participants in the Baseline condition, 1 for participants in the Negative condition, and 2 for participants in the Positive condition.

Measurement of Pandemic Status

Pandemic status was measured by the time period in which we conducted online experiments. We used the variable Time, coded 0 for data collected at T1, 1 for data collected at T2, and 2 for data collected at T3. We also generated two variables to represent the status change of the COVID-19 pandemic: Pandemic Status T1–T2 (coded 0 for the T1 pandemic stage and 1 for the T2 pandemic stage) and Pandemic Status T2–T3 (coded 0 for the T2 pandemic stage and 1 for the T3 pandemic stage).

Measurement of Social Anxiety

The degree of individual social anxiety (Socialanx) was measured by the Chinese version of the Social Interaction Anxiety Scale.50 The questionnaire includes 19 items, each answered on a five-point Likert scale (1 = not at all, 5 = extremely). Higher scores indicate more severe social anxiety. The Cronbach’s alpha reliability estimate in this study was 0.912.

Measurement of Self-Control

The degree of individual self-control (Selfcontrol) was measured by the Chinese version of the Self-Control Scale.51 The questionnaire includes 19 items, each answered on a five-point Likert scale (1 = not at all, 5 = extremely). As higher scores indicate less self-control, we reverse-scored the items. The Cronbach’s alpha reliability estimate in this study was 0.904.

Statistical Methods

Data were analyzed using SPSS-19.0 and Stata-17.0. The Chi-square test was used to compare the power of social information in different stages of the pandemic. We specified logit regression models to account for the dichotomous dependent variable (Power) and dichotomous independent variable (Pandemic Status T1–T2 or Pandemic Status T2–T3, in separate models) in different conditions (Baseline, Negative, Positive) separately. Thus, a total of six logit regressions were run in our analyses. Causal mediation analysis was conducted using the Stata medeff package to test whether social anxiety mediated the relationship between pandemic status and the nudge power of different types of social information. Finally, we employed a bootstrapping approach using the PROCESS macro for SPSS (Hayes, 2018) to test the significance of the moderating effect of self-control. In all these analyses, the demographics (Gender, Age, Sibling, Polity, Fam-income, Ind-expenditure) were treated as the control variables.

Results

Characteristics of Participants

The descriptive statistics in Table 1 show that 58.1% of participants were female (n = 796) and 41.9% were male (n = 575). The largest age group was 21–25 (41.9%), followed by 18–20 (27.2%), 26–30 (16.6%), and >30 (14.3%). Only 20.1% were members of the Communist Party. Regarding Fam-income, the largest group earned RMB 3001–6000 annually (36.0%), followed by > RMB 9000 (26.0%), RMB 0–3000 (25.5%), and RMB 6001–9000 (12.4%). Finally, for Ind-expenditure, the largest group spent RMB 1001–2000 per year (41.3%), followed by RMB 0–1000 (30.3%), > RMB 3000 (15.9%), and RMB 2001–3000 (12.5%). We also gave the descriptive statistics of the demographic variables at each pandemic status in Table 1. The results of the Kruskal–Wallis test show that there were no significant differences in the variables between samples in each period unless the sex of the participants with marginal significant difference.

Table 1 Descriptive Statistics of the Demographic Variables

The descriptive statistics in Table 2 indicate that the mean level of social anxiety among participants was 47.999. Looking at the changes over time, the level of social anxiety among participants tended to decrease as the pandemic eased and increase as the pandemic resurged. The mean level of self-control among participants was 51.600, and there was a tendency for self-control to decrease as the pandemic shifted from easing to resurging.

Table 2 Descriptive Statistics of Social-Anxiety and Self-Control

Participants’ Donation Behaviors at Different Pandemic Stages

We conducted Chi-square tests to examine whether pandemic status significantly influenced participants’ initial donation decisions. The results showed that participants’ donation behaviors did not significantly differ between the three pandemic stages (see Figure 2). At the outbreak stage (T1), 22.3% of participants chose to transfer money to the charitable account, 27.9% chose to transfer money out of the charitable account, and 49.8% chose not to adjust the balances of their personal and charitable accounts. During the trough stage (T2), 24.2% of participants transferred money to the charitable account, 27.6% transferred money out of the charitable account, and 48.1% made no transfer. Finally, at the resurgence stage (T3), 26.1% of participants increased the charitable account balance, 29.8% reduced its balance, and 44.1% left both account balances unchanged.

Figure 2 Participants’ donation behaviors at different pandemic stages.

The Impact of Pandemic Status on the Nudge Power of Social Information

We first conducted Chi-square tests to examine whether the nudge power of different types of social information significantly differed between pandemic stages. As reported in Table 3, the results show that the effect of social information in the Baseline condition stays low (T1: 10.1%; T2: 8.5%; T3: 9.5%) and does not significantly change with pandemic status (Fisher’s Exact Test, p = 1.000). The effect of negative social information (Negative condition) varies significantly with pandemic status (p = 0.019), as does the effect of positive social information (Positive condition) (p = 0.009).

Table 3 Test of the Nudge Power of Social Information Along with the Pandemic Status

To explore the trajectories of the nudge power of social information, we compared nudge power between T1 and T2 and between T2 and T3. The pairwise comparisons with Bonferroni adjustment show that nudge power in the Negative condition did not significantly differ between T1 and T2 (18.3% vs 14.4%, p>0.05) but did significantly differ between T2 and T3 (14.4% vs 27.6%, p < 0.05). Meanwhile, nudge power in the Positive condition differed significantly between T1 and T2 (31.8% vs 19.5%, p < 0.05) but remained stable from T2 to T3 (19.5% vs 22.8%, p >0.05; see Figure 3).

Figure 3 Effects of social information.

We conducted six logit regressions (two in each of the three conditions) to further examine the above results. As reported in Table 4, the regression results show that the nudge power of baseline social information does not vary with pandemic status (all p>0.1). The nudge power of negative social information remained stable from T1 to T2 (p>0.1) but significantly increased from T2 to T3 (p = 0.001). By contrast, the nudge power of positive social information significantly decreased fromT1 to T2 (p = 0.007) but then remained stable from T2 to T3 (p>0.1).

Table 4 Logit Regression Results

The Mediating Role of Social Anxiety

Causal mediation analyses were conducted to test whether social anxiety mediated between pandemic status and the nudge power of social information. The results in Table 4 show that pandemic status was only significantly related to the power of social information in two situations: the status change from trough to resurgence in the Negative condition and the status change from outbreak to trough in the Positive condition. Thus, we mainly investigated the mediating role of social anxiety in these two situations. Table 5 reports the causal mediation analyses results. The average causal mediation effect of social anxiety under the Negative condition was significant (b = 0.150, 95% CI = [0.075, 0.228]) and accounted for 62.2% of the total effect. Social anxiety also significantly mediated the relationship between pandemic status change from T1 to T2 and the nudge power of positive social information: the average causal mediation effect was significant (b = −0.074, 95% CI = [−0.107, −0.043]) and accounted for 55.4% of the total effect.

Table 5 Results for the Mediating Role of Social Anxiety

Moderated Mediation Effect Test

We used Model 8 in the PROCESS macro for SPSS to examine the moderated mediation effect. The macro also generated a 5000-bootstrap confidence interval (without zero) for the significance test. We tested the moderated mediation effect in two situations: pandemic status change from T2 to T3 in the Negative condition (see Table 6) and pandemic status change from T1 to T2 in the Positive condition (see Table 7).

Table 6 Results for the Moderated Mediation Effect of Self-Control in the Negative Condition

Table 7 Results for the Moderated Mediation Effect of Self-Control in the Positive Condition

The results in Table 6 indicate that the interaction term of pandemic status change from T2 to T3 and self-control showed significant effects on social anxiety (B = −0.675, p < 0.001, 95% CI = [−0.948, −0.402]) and the nudge power of negative social information (B = −0.174, p = 0.001, 95% CI = [−0.271, −0.076]). These results demonstrate that, in the Negative condition, both the association between Pandemic Status T2–T3 and Socialanx and the association between Pandemic Status T2–T3 and Power were moderated by self-control.

To further understand the moderating role of self-control, we tested the conditional direct effect and conditional indirect effect of self-control at the mean level, one standard deviation below the mean, and one standard deviation above the mean. For individuals with low self-control ability, Pandemic Status T2–T3 had a significant direct effect on the power of negative social information. However, for those with median and high self-control ability, the direct association between Pandemic Status T2–T3 and the power of negative social information disappeared. Conversely, for individuals with low and median self-control ability, Pandemic Status T2–T3 was significantly related to the power of negative social information through the mediator social anxiety. For those with high self-control ability, however, the indirect effect Pandemic Status T2–T3→Socialanx→Power disappeared. The index of moderated mediation indicated that self-control played a significant role (Index = −0.122, [BootLLCI, BootULCI] = [−0.250, −0.066]).

The results in Table 7 indicate that the interaction term of pandemic status change from T1 to T2 and self-control showed significant effects on social anxiety (B = −0.543, p < 0.001, 95% CI = [0.349, 0.738]) and the nudge power of positive social information (B = −0.101, p = 0.001, 95% CI = [0.051, 0.150]). These results demonstrate that, in the Positive condition, both the association between Pandemic Status T1–T2 and Socialanx and the association between Pandemic Status T1–T2 and Power were moderated by self-control.

The conditional direct effect and conditional indirect effect of self-control were tested at the mean level, one standard deviation below the mean, and one standard deviation above the mean. The results indicate that the direct effect of Pandemic Status T1–T2 on the nudge power of positive social information was significantly negative for low self-control (B = −1.815, [LLCI, ULCI] = [0.051, 0.150]), non-significant for median self-control (B = −0.519, [LLCI, ULCI] = [−1.086, 0.047]), and significantly positive for high self-control (B = 0.776, [LLCI, ULCI] = [0.034, 1.518]). Meanwhile, the indirect effect Pandemic Status T1–T2→Socialanx→Power changed from significant to non-significant with the advancement of self-control ability. That is, the indirect effect Pandemic Status T1–T2→Socialanx→Power was significantly negative for low self-control (B = −0.950, [LLCI, ULCI] = [−1.436, −0.622]) and median self-control (B = −0.451, [LLCI, ULCI] = [−0.725, −0.264]), and non-significant for high self-control (B = 0.049, [LLCI, ULCI] = [−0.245, 0.369]). Overall, we found that self-control moderated both paths between Pandemic Status T1–T2 and the nudge power of positive social information: the indirect effect via social anxiety and the direct effect.

Discussion

Since the emergence of the COVID-19 pandemic in late 2019, the world has experienced its impact for three years, with significant economic consequences. Charitable donations have become a crucial aspect of managing the pandemic and rebuilding the economy. As a result, it is essential to understand the factors that influence people’s donation decisions during sudden and chronic emergencies. Given that individuals are sensitive to positive and negative social information, we used an adapted give-or-take donation game to investigate how information about peers’ positive and negative donation behaviors affects an individual’s donation decisions. We were particularly interested in how the power of social information changed at various stages of the COVID-19 pandemic. Thus, we conducted cross-sectional surveys at three different time points: April-June 2020, February-March 2021, and May 2022.

The findings indicate that people’s initial donation behavior did not vary with the pandemic situation. Two potential reasons may explain this outcome. First, factors such as personal beliefs, values, and education level may impact an individual’s donation behavior more than external events such as disasters. Elliott and Pais52 found that charitable giving is a relatively stable behavior rather than a response to external stimuli. The second reason may be that while disasters can increase people’s willingness to donate, they can also worsen their economic situation and decrease their willingness to donate.53 These two factors may offset each other, resulting in no significant change in people’s donation behaviors.

Our study found that the effectiveness of social information in nudging individuals’ donation behaviors changed according to the pandemic stage. During the outbreak stage (T1), 31.8% of individuals increased their initial donations when presented with positive social information, while during the trough stage (T2), only 19.5% did so. This difference was statistically significant. However, during the resurgence stage (T3), 22.8% increased their initial donations, with no significant change from the trough stage. On the other hand, negative social information prompted individuals to lower their initial donations, with 18.3% and 14.4% doing so during the outbreak and trough stages, respectively. In the resurgence stage, there was a significant increase in the proportion of individuals (27.6%) reducing their initial donations. These findings are consistent with previous research that positive crowd behaviors can increase prosociality, while negative behaviors can decrease it.54

Our study revealed that the effectiveness of social information as a nudge mechanism is situation dependent and linked to the social context of individuals.55 Previous research has shown that nudge mechanisms can have unintended consequences if not appropriately situated in people’s social contexts.56,57 To address this challenge, Hauser et al9 proposed the Beliefs-Barriers-Context (BBC) model, which explains why replicating the effects of many nudge mechanisms is difficult and offers guidance on their design. The BBC model emphasizes the importance of context, encompassing both physical and social settings, in determining the effectiveness of a nudge mechanism. Similarly, Ewert et al58 developed a behaviourally-informed, integrated conceptual model of the policy process that takes into account individual attitudes and behaviors in various contexts at the meso and macro levels. This model highlights how institutional, socio-economic, cultural, and environmental contexts can shape cognitive processing and ultimately influence the effectiveness of a nudge mechanism.55

The COVID-19 pandemic has drastically altered the socio-economic landscape and continues to impact people’s behavior and psychological well-being.59–61 Changes in the social context affect how individuals perceive the same social information. This study revealed different trends in the nudge power of positive and negative social information as the status of the pandemic changed over time. Compared to the outbreak stage, the trough stage brought a significant decline in the power of positive social information but no significant change in the power of negative social information. However, during the resurgence phase, the influence of negative social information significantly increased, while there was no change in the power of positive social information. These results suggest that changes in the pandemic situation have had a dissimilar impact on the power of positive and negative social information.

When the pandemic initially broke out, people came together to help prevent and control its spread. Disasters have been found to change individuals’ prosocial preferences and increase their sensitivity to positive social information.15,62 For example, after the 2011 earthquake and tsunami in Japan, individuals showed a greater willingness to help others and became more responsive to positive social information.63 Therefore, during the outbreak stage of the COVID-19 pandemic, people were more likely to be influenced by positive social information and examples, leading to an increase in their charitable behavior.54 Lotti and Pethiyagoda64 argue that during a negative socio-economic shock such as COVID-19, moral motives are more influential, which could lead to an increase in charitable behavior. However, as the pandemic subsided, the power of positive social information decreased.35

Studies have also shown that people’s economic situation gradually worsens, negative psychological pressure likely accumulates, and selfishness and anti-social behavior increase as the COVID-19 pandemic lasts long or gets serious.65 These negative effects might eventually outweigh the positive effect of pandemic on prosociality.66 As resources become scarcer due to the pandemic’s persistence and resurgences, competition between individuals can increase, leading to a decrease in prosocial behavior.67,68 Consequently, people become more concerned about and susceptible to negative social information.25 Although people’s initial donation behavior did not change in line with the pandemic situation which is consistent with the findings of Grimalda et al,69 those receiving negative information at the resurgence stage were more likely to be influenced by peers’ negative donation behavior.

We found that social anxiety played a mediating role between COVID-19 pandemic status and the nudge power of social information. It is undeniable that the pandemic has had a dramatic impact on people’s mental state,70–72 including levels of social anxiety.35 While quarantine policies have been effective in reducing infections,36,37 social isolation resulting from them has likely worsened social anxiety.73 The mediating effects of social anxiety were not symmetrical for positive and negative social information. For the change from outbreak to trough stage, social anxiety only significantly mediated the effect of positive social information. Conversely, for the change in COVID-19 status from trough to resurgence stage, only the effect on negative social information was significantly mediated. Social anxiety affects people’s behavior mainly by generating expected external evaluation pressure.74,75 At the outbreak stage, prosocial behaviors and preferences were prevalent,15,54,62 so actions contrary to positive social information were expected to receive stronger negative external evaluation. When the pandemic abated, however, the balance was restored. Therefore, from outbreak to trough stage, social anxiety only mediated the relationship between pandemic status and the power of positive social information. As the pandemic persisted, however, individuals suffering negative impacts on their financial situation or physical and/or mental health behaved less prosocially.66 From trough to resurgence stage, individuals might thus have expected to receive a stronger negative external evaluation when behaving contrary to negative social information. On this basis, social anxiety mediated the relationship between pandemic status and the nudge power of negative social information.

This study also indicated that self-control moderated the direct effect of COVID-19 status on the power of social information and the indirect effect via social anxiety. Individuals with high self-control are more resistant to external temptations, distractions, and pressures,47,51 and have higher emotional stability.48 Therefore, those with high self-control are more capable of maintaining their behaviors and mental states when facing changes in the severity of the COVID-19 pandemic, making self-control a significant negative moderator.

Limitations and Future Directions

This research has several limitations. First, previous studies have found that the influence of social information depends on the source. Abel and Brown54 found that information on the same behaviors by private and public role models had opposite effects. This study focused only on social information from peers. Therefore, future studies should consider other social information sources, such as representatives of government or non-profit organizations and strangers. Second, people’s donation behaviors have been found to differ for different charitable funds, including during the COVID-19 pandemic.64 This study only set one charitable fund that the participants can donate to. This design cannot eliminate the impact of the participants’ natural preferences to the given fund and investigate the different donation behaviors to different funds. These research directions should also be expanded. Third, although we collected three sets of data at different stages of the pandemic, the research design was cross-sectional, with three different samples. A longitudinal approach should be used in future studies to enable causal inferences and gain a more fruitful overview of how the power of social information changes at different stages of the COVID-19 pandemic. Finally, all our study participants were drawn from a Chinese cultural background, which limits the generalizability of the findings.7 Future research should test whether the findings hold for other cultural groups.

Conclusion

Our research provides new insight into the research on the nudge power of social information on individuals’ donation behaviors by investigating their situation dependence and the underlying psychological mechanism. The findings suggest that the nudge power of positive social information about peer examples’ prosocial behaviors decreased, whereas the nudge power of negative social information increased in line with the status of the pandemic. The results also indicated that social anxiety played a significant mediating role in the relationship between COVID-19 status and nudge power of social information. Moreover, self-control moderated the direct effect of COVID-19 status on power of social information and the indirect effect of this relation through social anxiety. These findings have important implications for understanding the situational factors that influence individuals’ prosocial behaviors and how social information can be used to nudge them towards more desirable outcomes. Specifically, the results suggest that during a pandemic, positive social information about prosocial behaviors may be less effective, while negative social information may be more effective in motivating individuals to behave in a certain way. Thus, during disasters, negative social information should be suppressed and positive social information should be highlighted. Furthermore, policy makers should consider using negative social information to nudge individuals towards prosocial behavior during the disasters. The situational factors and the psychological factors, such as social anxiety and self-control, should also be taken into account in designing nudge policies aimed at promoting prosocial behavior.

Data Sharing Statement

Data supporting the findings presented in the current study will be available from the corresponding author (Kehan Li, email: [email protected]) upon request.

Ethical Approval

This study was performed according to the Declaration of Helsinki for Medical Research involving Human Subjects. All participants gave their informed consent for inclusion before they participated in the study. On the title page of the survey, all participants were told anonymity was assured, why the research was being conducted, and how the data they were providing were being stored and used. Those who are worried about the use of the data or the purpose of this study could refuse to participate in the study or withdraw from the study at any time to ensure their right to confidentiality. All the procedures were approved by the Ethics Committees of Shandong University and Shandong University of Finance and Economics (SDU-SE-IRB 2020-010).

Acknowledgment

We would like to thank all participants in the study.

Funding

This research was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (72202119), National Social Science Foundation of China (22&ZD150), and Natural Science Foundation of Shandong Province (ZR2021QG055, ZR2022QG067).

Disclosure

The authors report no conflicts of interest in this work.

References

1. Singhal T. A review of coronavirus disease-2019 (COVID-19). Indian J Pediatr. 2020;87(4):281–286. doi:10.1007/s12098-020-03263-6

2. Danylyshyn B. The peculiarities of economic crisis due to COVID-19 pandemic in a developing country: case of Ukraine. Problems Perspect Manage. 2020;18(2):13–22. doi:10.21511/ppm.18(2).2020.02

3. Dube B. Rural online learning in the context of COVID 19 in South Africa: evoking an inclusive education approach. REMIE. 2020;10(2):135–157. doi:10.4471/remie.2020.5607

4. Brooks SK, Webster RK, Smith LE, et al. The psychological impact of quarantine and how to reduce it: rapid review of the evidence. Lancet. 2020;395(10227):912–920. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30460-8

5. Parmet WE, Sinha MS. Covid-19—the law and limits of quarantine. N Engl J Med. 2020;382(15):e28. doi:10.1056/NEJMp2004211

6. Lee CC, Chen YJ, Wu PL, et al. An unintended consequence of social distance regulations: COVID-19 social distancing promotes the desire for money. Br J Psychol. 2021;112(4):866–878. doi:10.1111/bjop.12497

7. Li L, Zhang Q, Wang X, et al. Characterizing the propagation of situational information in social media during covid-19 epidemic: a case study on weibo. IEEE Transact Comput Soc Syst. 2020;7(2):556–562. doi:10.1109/TCSS.2020.2980007

8. Bicchieri C, Dimant E. Nudging with care: the risks and benefits of social information. Public Choice. 2022;191(3):443–464. doi:10.1007/s11127-019-00684-6

9. Hauser OP, Gino F, Norton MI. Budging beliefs, nudging behaviour. Mind Soc. 2018;17(1):15–26. doi:10.1007/s11299-019-00200-9

10. Beshears J, Kosowsky H. Nudging: progress to date and future directions. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process. 2020;161:3–19. doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2020.09.001

11. Croson R, Shang J. Limits of the effect of social information on the voluntary provision of public goods: evidence from field experiments. Econ Inq. 2013;51(1):473–477. doi:10.1111/j.1465-7295.2012.00468.x

12. Hysenbelli D, Rubaltelli E, Rumiati R. Others’ opinions count, but not all of them: anchoring to ingroup versus outgroup members’ behavior in charitable giving. Judgm Decis Mak. 2013;8(6):678–690. doi:10.1017/S193029750000471X

13. Smith S, Windmeijer F, Wright E. Peer effects in charitable giving: evidence from the (running) field. Econ J. 2015;125(585):1053–1071. doi:10.1111/ecoj.12114

14. Van Teunenbroek C, Bekkers R. Follow the crowd: social information and crowdfunding donations in a large field experiment. J Behav Public Admin. 2020;3(1):1–17. doi:10.30636/jbpa.31.87

15. Chen Y, Harper FM, Konstan J, et al. Social comparisons and contributions to online communities: a field experiment on movielens. Am Econom Rev. 2010;100(4):1358–1398. doi:10.1257/aer.100.4.1358

16. d’Adda G, Capraro V, Tavoni M. Push, don’t nudge: behavioral spillovers and policy instruments. Econ Lett. 2017;154:92–95. doi:10.1016/j.econlet.2017.02.029

17. Murphy JJ, Batmunkh N, Nilsson B, et al. The impact of social information on the voluntary provision of public goods: a replication study. Res Exp Econ. 2015;18:41–50. doi:10.1108/S0193-230620150000018002

18. Meyer A, Yang G. How much versus who: which social norms information is more effective? Appl Econ. 2016;48(5):389–401. doi:10.1080/00036846.2015.1080803

19. Kubo T, Shoji Y, Tsuge T, et al. Voluntary contributions to hiking trail maintenance: evidence from a field experiment in a national park, Japan. Ecol Econom. 2018;144:124–128. doi:10.1016/j.ecolecon.2017.07.032

20. Park S, Shin J, Ponti G. The influence of anonymous peers on prosocial behavior. PLoS One. 2017;12(10):e0185521. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0185521

21. Van Teunenbroek C, Bekkers R, Beersma B. Look to others before you leap: a systematic literature review of social information effects on donation amounts. Nonprofit Voluntary Sector Quart. 2020;49(1):53–73. doi:10.1177/0899764019869537

22. Tian Y, Konrath S. The effects of similarity on charitable giving in donor–donor dyads: a systematic literature review. Voluntas. 2021;32(2):316–339. doi:10.1007/s11266-019-00165-w

23. Dimant E. Contagion of pro-and anti-social behavior among peers and the role of social proximity. J Econ Psychol. 2019;73:66–88. doi:10.1016/j.joep.2019.04.009

24. Shen Z, Zhong Z, Xie J, et al. The effects of information-seeking behaviors on risk perception during the COVID-19 pandemic: a cross-sectional correlational survey. Psychol Res Behav Manag. 2022;15:1707–1719. doi:10.2147/PRBM.S368537

25. Marchiori DR, Adriaanse MA, De Ridder DTD. Unresolved questions in nudging research: putting the psychology back in nudging. Soc Personal Psychol Compass. 2017;11(1):e12297. doi:10.1111/spc3.12297

26. Goldstein NJ, Cialdini RB, Griskevicius V. A room with a viewpoint: using social norms to motivate environmental conservation in hotels. J Consumer Res. 2008;35(3):472–482. doi:10.1086/586910

27. Bohner G, Schlüter LE. A room with a viewpoint revisited: descriptive norms and hotel guests’ towel reuse behavior. PLoS One. 2014;9(8):e104086. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0104086

28. Stroebe W, Strack F. The alleged crisis and the illusion of exact replication. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2014;9(1):59–71. doi:10.1177/1745691613514450

29. Wang C, Pan R, Wan X, et al. A longitudinal study on the mental health of general population during the COVID-19 epidemic in China. Brain Behav Immun. 2020;87:40–48. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2020.04.028

30. Van Dessel P, Boddez Y, Hughes S. Nudging societally relevant behavior by promoting cognitive inferences. Sci Rep. 2022;12(1):1–11. doi:10.1038/s41598-022-12964-1

31. Wang W, Li S, Li J, et al. The COVID-19 pandemic changes the nudging effect of social information on individuals’ blood donation intention. Front Psychol. 2021;12:736002. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2021.736002

32. Dodge KA. Emotion and social information processing. Dev Emotion Regulat Dysregul. 1991;1:159–181.

33. Lemerise EA, Arsenio WF. An integrated model of emotion processes and cognition in social information processing. Child Dev. 2000;71(1):107–118. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00124

34. McPherson KE, McAloney-Kocaman K, McGlinchey E, et al. Longitudinal analysis of the UK COVID-19 Psychological Wellbeing Study: trajectories of anxiety, depression and COVID-19-related stress symptomology. Psychiatry Res. 2021;304:114138. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2021.114138

35. Li S, Liu X, Li J. The contagion of donation behaviors changes along with the abatement of the covid-19 pandemic: an intertemporal survey experiment. Front Psychol. 2021;12:585128. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2021.585128

36. Nussbaumer-Streit B, Mayr V, Dobrescu AI, et al. Quarantine alone or in combination with other public health measures to control COVID‐19: a rapid review. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2020;(9):CD013574. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD013574.pub2

37. Memon Z, Qureshi S, Memon BR. Assessing the role of quarantine and isolation as control strategies for COVID-19 outbreak: a case study. Chaos Solitons Fractals. 2021;144:110655. doi:10.1016/j.chaos.2021.110655

38. Zheng L, Miao M, Lim JY, et al. Is lockdown bad for social anxiety in COVID-19 regions?: a national study in the SOR perspective. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;17(12):4561. doi:10.3390/ijerph17124561

39. Morrissette M. School closures and social anxiety during the COVID-19 pandemic. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2021;60(1):6–7. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2020.08.436

40. Peschard V, Philippot P. Social anxiety and information processing biases: an integrated theoretical perspective. Cogn Emot. 2016;30(4):762–777. doi:10.1080/02699931.2015.1028335

41. Gu R, Ao X, Mo L, et al. Neural correlates of negative expectancy and impaired social feedback processing in social anxiety. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2020;15(3):285–291. doi:10.1093/scan/nsaa038

42. Baumeister RF, Vohs KD, Tice DM. The strength model of self-control. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2007;16(6):351–355. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00534.x

43. Paschke LM, Dörfel D, Steimke R, et al. Individual differences in self-reported self-control predict successful emotion regulation. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2016;11(8):1193–1204. doi:10.1093/scan/nsw036

44. Hoyle RH, Davisson EK. Varieties of self-control and their personality correlates. Handbook Self Regulat. 2016;3:396–413. doi:10.31234/osf.io/2eqcz

45. Burkley E, Anderson D, Curtis J. You wore me down: self‐control strength and social influence. Soc Personal Psychol Compass. 2011;5(7):487–499. doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2011.00367.x

46. Wills TA, Pokhrel P, Morehouse E, et al. Behavioral and emotional regulation and adolescent substance use problems: a test of moderation effects in a dual-process model. Psychol Addict Behav. 2011;25(2):279–292. doi:10.1037/a0022870

47. Coventry LM, Jeske D, Blythe JM, et al. Personality and social framing in privacy decision-making: a study on cookie acceptance. Front Psychol. 2016;7:1341. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01341

48. Daly M, Baumeister RF, Delaney L, et al. Self-control and its relation to emotions and psychobiology: evidence from a Day Reconstruction Method study. J Behav Med. 2014;37(1):81–93. doi:10.1007/s10865-012-9470-9

49. Oaten M, Cheng K. Improved self-control: the benefits of a regular program of academic study. Basic Appl Soc Psych. 2006;28(1):1–16. doi:10.1207/s15324834basp2801_1

50. Mattick RP, Clarke JC. Development and validation of measures of social phobia scrutiny fear and social interaction anxiety. Behav Res Ther. 1998;36(4):455–470. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(97)10031-6

51. Tangney JP, Baumeister RF, Boone AL. High self‐control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. J Pers. 2004;72(2):271–324. doi:10.1111/j.0022-3506.2004.00263.x

52. Elliott JR, Pais J. Race, class, and Hurricane Katrina: social differences in human responses to disaster. Soc Sci Res. 2006;35(2):295–321. doi:10.1016/j.ssresearch.2006.02.003

53. Bekkers R, Wiepking P. A literature review of empirical studies of philanthropy: eight mechanisms that drive charitable giving. Nonprofit Voluntary Sector Quart. 2011;40(5):924–973. doi:10.1177/0899764010380927

54. Abel M, Brown W. Prosocial behavior in the time of COVID-19: the effect of private and public role models. J Behav Exp Econ. 2022;101:101942. doi:10.1016/j.socec.2022.101942

55. Banerjee S, John P. Nudge plus: incorporating reflection into behavioral public policy. Behav Public Policy. 2021;5717(332):1–16. doi:10.1017/bpp.2021.6

56. Bovens L. Real nudge. Eur J Risk Regulat. 2012;3(1):43–46. doi:10.1017/S1867299X00001793

57. Bhanot SP, Linos E. Behavioral public administration: past, present, and future. Public Adm Rev. 2020;80(1):168–171. doi:10.1111/puar.13129

58. Ewert B, Loer K, Thomann E. Beyond nudge: advancing the state-of-The-art of behavioural public policy and administration. Policy Polit. 2021;49(1):3–23. doi:10.1332/030557320X15987279194319

59. Bavel JJV, Baicker K, Boggio PS, et al. Using social and behavioural science to support COVID-19 pandemic response. Nat Human Behav. 2020;4(5):460–471. doi:10.1038/s41562-020-0884-z

60. Valaskova K, Durana P, Adamko P. Changes in consumers’ purchase patterns as a consequence of the COVID-19 pandemic. Mathematics. 2021;9(15):1788. doi:10.3390/math9151788

61. Brodeur A, Gray D, Islam A, et al. A literature review of the economics of COVID‐19. J Econ Surv. 2021;35(4):1007–1044. doi:10.1111/joes.12423

62. Oosterhof L, Heuvelman A, Peters O. Donation to disaster relief campaigns: underlying social cognitive factors exposed. Eval Program Plann. 2009;32(2):148–157. doi:10.1016/j.evalprogplan.2008.10.006

63. Aknin LB, Barrington-Leigh CP, Dunn EW, et al. Prosocial spending and well-being: cross-cultural evidence for a psychological universal. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2013;104(4):635–652. doi:10.1037/a0031578

64. Lotti L, Pethiyagoda S. Generosity during COVID-19: investigating socioeconomic shocks and game framing. Human Soc Sci Commun. 2022;9(1):1–10. doi:10.1057/s41599-022-01200-w

65. Buso IM, De Caprariis S, Di Cagno D, et al. The effects of COVID-19 lockdown on fairness and cooperation: evidence from a lablike experiment. Econ Lett. 2020;196:109577. doi:10.1016/j.econlet.2020.109577

66. Adena M, Harke J. COVID-19 and pro-sociality: how do donors respond to local pandemic severity, increased salience, and media coverage? Exp Econ. 2022;25(3):824–844. doi:10.1007/s10683-022-09753-y

67. Nowak MA. Five rules for the evolution of cooperation. Science. 2006;314(5805):1560–1563. doi:10.1126/science.1133755

68. Becchetti L, Castriota S, Conzo P. Disaster, aid, and preferences: the long-run impact of the tsunami on giving in Sri Lanka. World Dev. 2017;94:157–173. doi:10.1016/j.worlddev.2016.12.014

69. Grimalda G, Buchan NR, Ozturk OD, et al. Exposure to COVID-19 is associated with increased altruism, particularly at the local level. Sci Rep. 2021;11(1):1–14. doi:10.1038/s41598-021-97234-2

70. Keneth IK, Mujinya R, Bogere P, et al. Pandemic panic and anxiety in developing countries. Embracing One Health offers practical strategies in management of COVID-19 for Africa. Pan Afr Med J. 2020;35(Suppl 2). doi:10.11604/pamj.2020.35.3.22637

71. Talevi D, Socci V, Carai M, et al. Mental health outcomes of the CoViD-19 pandemic. Riv Psichiatr. 2020;55(3):137–144. doi:10.1708/3382.33569

72. Pfefferbaum B, North CS. Mental health and the Covid-19 pandemic. N Engl J Med. 2020;383(6):510–512. doi:10.1093/qjmed/hcaa110

73. Kashdan TB, Goodman FR, Machell KA, et al. A contextual approach to experiential avoidance and social anxiety: evidence from an experimental interaction and daily interactions of people with social anxiety disorder. Emotion. 2014;14(4):769–781. doi:10.1037/a0035935

74. Van der Molen MJW, Poppelaars ES, Van Hartingsveldt CTA, et al. Fear of negative evaluation modulates electrocortical and behavioral responses when anticipating social evaluative feedback. Front Hum Neurosci. 2014;7(936):1–12. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00936

75. Cao J, Gu R, Bi X, et al. Unexpected acceptance? Patients with social anxiety disorder manifest their social expectancy in ERPs during social feedback processing. Front Psychol. 2015;6:1745. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01745

Creative Commons License © 2023 The Author(s). This work is published and licensed by Dove Medical Press Limited. The full terms of this license are available at https://www.dovepress.com/terms.php and incorporate the Creative Commons Attribution - Non Commercial (unported, v3.0) License. By accessing the work you hereby accept the Terms. Non-commercial uses of the work are permitted without any further permission from Dove Medical Press Limited, provided the work is properly attributed. For permission for commercial use of this work, please see paragraphs 4.2 and 5 of our Terms.