Back to Journals » Clinical Interventions in Aging » Volume 12

Bilingualism as a strategy to delay the onset of Alzheimer’s disease

Authors Klimova B , Valis M, Kuca K 

Received 3 July 2017

Accepted for publication 3 August 2017

Published 19 October 2017 Volume 2017:12 Pages 1731—1737

DOI https://doi.org/10.2147/CIA.S145397

Checked for plagiarism Yes

Review by Single anonymous peer review

Peer reviewer comments 2

Editor who approved publication: Dr Richard Walker



Blanka Klimova,1,2 Martin Valis,2 Kamil Kuca3,4

1Department of Applied Linguistics, Faculty of Informatics and Management, University of Hradec Kralove, 2Department of Neurology, 3Biomedical Research Centre, University Hospital Hradec Kralove, 4Department of Chemistry, Faculty of Science, University of Hradec Kralove, Hradec Kralove, Czech Republic

Abstract: The purpose of this study is to explore original studies which provide evidence about the effects of bilingualism on the delay of the onset of dementia, specifically Alzheimer’s disease (AD). A literature review was conducted in the world’s acknowledged databases: Web of Science, Scopus, and MEDLINE. Altogether, 14 original studies focusing on the research topic were detected. These included six prospective cohort studies and eight retrospective studies. Both types of studies suggest different conclusions. The findings from the prospective cohort studies state that there is no association between bilingualism and the delay of the onset of AD, while the retrospective studies claim the opposite. Despite the negative results of the prospective cohort studies, more research should be conducted on bilingualism and its impact on the delay of the onset of AD, since the brain studies have brought positive findings as far as the enhancement of cognitive reserve is concerned.

Keywords: older people, evidence, effectiveness, review, cognition

Introduction

Aging and dementia

According to the recent report of the United Nations1 on the aging process of the world’s population, the number of older people aged 60+ years has risen considerably. Globally, the number of these elderly people is growing faster than the number of people in any other age group. This accelerating trend in the rise of older population groups inevitably results in serious economic and social changes accompanied with a number of aging diseases such as dementia.

Dementia, specifically Alzheimer’s disease (AD), together with heart diseases, cancer, and respiratory diseases, is one of the most common causes of death among older people.2 Globally, dementia affects around 58 million people, and it is estimated that the number of older people with dementia would triple by 2050, since each year about 9.9 million new dementia cases are diagnosed worldwide.3,4

Dementia is a syndrome of deterioration of cognitive functions that interferes with a patient’s everyday life. This cognitive impairment has an enormous impact not only on the patient’s health and his/her self-care but also on the costs of care, either at home or in a residential establishment.57 Therefore, there is sustainable effort to prevent cognitive decline which is the foremost sign of this neurological disorder. However, currently, there is no effective pharmacological treatment for the cognitive decline.8 Thus, there is an increasing interest in non-pharmacological alternative approaches which may maintain or even increase the so-called cognitive reserve and enable healthy older people to delay the deterioration of cognitive functions.

Cognitive reserve, its enhancement, and bilingualism

Cognitive reserve can be defined as the resilience to neuropathological damage of the brain.9 It is assumed that this is the result of experience-based neural changes which are caused by a physically and mentally stimulating lifestyle.10 Research11 indicates that especially the differences in cognitive reserve are affected by environmental enrichment. This means that although some people have signs of brain atrophy and neuronal loss, thanks to their cognitive reserve, they do not have visible difficulties in cognitive functioning.

A research study12 revealed that 60% of general cognitive ability is of genetic origin, but there are some non-pharmacological activities such as performing physical and mental activities, which play an important role in its enhancement as well.13 Antoniou et al10 claim that some of these non-pharmacological activities which are aimed at the stimulation of the cognitive functions might delay the cognitive decline.

One of these mental activities, which may also contribute to the enhancement of cognitive reserve, is bilingualism, that is, the ability to speak more than one language. In fact, bilingualism enables constant shifting between languages, with their distinct sounds, words, concepts, and grammatical rules, and offers effective training of executive functions such as attention, switching, inhibition, and monitoring. All these contribute to a higher cognitive reserve.14 Thus, bilingualism is considered to be connected with cognitive advantage,15 since research16 has shown that language does not only reflect the mind but also modify the brain to certain degree even in older age. Bak et al,17 Goral et al,18 and Luk et al19 report that bilingual people may enhance their cognitive reserve at later age, thanks to the use of two languages, and the so-called mental flexibility. Schweitzer et al20 argue that bilingualism is a clear case of an environmental factor that helps produce cognitive reserve, although it might also affect the brain and its development and organization, and contribute to brain reserve. There are several brain studies which confirm that bilingual people usually have greater gray matter volume than monolingual individuals, which has a positive impact on the functional connectivity between gray matter regions.2124 Therefore, bilingualism seems to protect against the cognitive decline that occurs during aging. This has also been confirmed by the most recent study by Estanga et al,25 who conclude that bilingualism influences cognitive reserve, especially improving executive and visual-spatial functions.

Bialystok et al26 claim that thanks to better mental flexibility, bilingual people have the ability to adapt to constant changes and process information effectively and adaptively. Furthermore, Lee and Tzeng27 indicate that effective connectivity, thanks to second language learning, improves the capacity for language processing and general executive control by reorganizing neural circuitries. This even concerns older learners whose age is considered to be far from the so-called critical period of language acquisition. These findings have also been confirmed by Schlegel et al28 who have revealed that the brain maintains its plasticity in adult age. In their study on language learning with 11 English speakers studying Chinese for nine months, they have brought evidence that white matter in older individuals might still experience considerable changes which have a positive impact on foreign language learning in aging. Bialystok29 points out that the age in second language acquisition is not such a significant factor, but the length of exposure to the target language is important.

The purpose of this study is to explore original studies which provide evidence about the effects of bilingualism on the delay of the onset of AD.

Methods

The methodology of this study was based on Moher et al.30 A literature review was conducted in Web of Science, Scopus, and MEDLINE databases with the goal of identifying research studies on the basis of the following key words: bilingualism AND cognitive decline, bilingualism AND dementia, and bilingualism AND AD. This review identified articles published during 2007 to May 2017.

Most of the studies on the research topic were identified in the Web of Science (146), followed by MEDLINE (129) and Scopus (104). Altogether, 379 publications were detected in the databases. The titles of all studies were checked in order to confirm whether they focused on the research topic and to eliminate duplicates. Ninety-eight studies were used for further analysis, in which the authors checked the content of the abstracts. Sixty studies were selected for the full-text analysis, out of which the findings of 14 studies were then used for the detailed analysis of the research topic (Figure 1).

Figure 1 Results of the selection procedure.

A study was included if it matched the corresponding period, that is, from 2007 up to May 2017. The selection period started with the year of 2007, since this is the year when the first findings on the efficacy of bilingualism on the delay of the onset of dementia appeared.26 Furthermore, a study was included if it involved older people and focused on the topic of bilingualism, that is, the ability to speak and write more than one language, and its effect on the onset of AD. The studies which focused only on AD were included, while the studies concentrating on other, less frequent dementias, such as frontotemporal dementia31 or Parkinson’s diseases, were excluded.32 The studies18,33 that compared the performance of bilinguals to that of monolinguals on tasks such as nonverbal tasks, assumed to measure executive functioning, were excluded, as well as multilingual studies.34,35 The brain studies2125 were also excluded. Only studies written in English were included.

Findings

Altogether, 14 original studies focusing on the research topic were detected. These included six prospective cohort studies (Table 1) and eight retrospective studies (Table 2). All studies except two38,45 compared both monolingual and bilingual language groups. Tables 1 and 2 provide an overview of the main findings on bilingualism and its effect on the delay of the onset of AD. The studies are presented in the alphabetical order of their first author.

Table 1 An overview of the prospective cohort studies on bilingualism and its effect on the delay of the onset of AD
Abbreviations: AD, Alzheimer’s disease; n-NESs, non-native English speakers; MCI, mild cognitive impairment.

Table 2 An overview of the retrospective and cross-sectional studies on bilingualism and its effect on the delay of the onset of AD
Abbreviations: AD, Alzheimer’s disease; MCI, mild cognitive impairment; D-KEFS, Delis–Kaplan Executive Function System; aMCI, amnestic mild cognitive impairment.

The research period of the prospective studies ranges between 5 and 50 years. Overall, the number of participants in these six studies was 8,233. All six studies in fact met the criteria for the evaluation of the quality of cohort studies set by the Newcastle–Ottawa Scale,48 since they applied evidence-based measurement of their outcomes, such as a battery of cognitive performance tests, clinical examinations, or Cox regression. Furthermore, the followed-up period lasted 5 years at minimum, the clinical samples were derived from community samples and not referrals to specialist memory clinics, and most importantly, the age of the onset of AD was determined by standardized clinical examinations and not by subjective retrospective self-reports. In some studies, however, the self-reports were used for the assessment of language proficiency.36,39,40 As Zahodne et al40 indicate, this should not have any effect on the overall results. Apart from two studies,17,38 the findings suggest that there is no association between bilingualism and the delay of the onset of dementia, specifically AD.

On the contrary, the results of all retrospective studies with one exception43 indicate that bilingualism might delay the onset of cognitive decline, specifically AD, by approximately 4.5 years. However, the number of subjects in these studies is lower than in the prospective studies. Altogether, 1,502 patients participated in the research, ranging from 44 to 648 patients. Bakker49 points out that in order to attain desirable levels of power, at least 139 subjects should be involved in each language group. Criterion for bilingualism in these studies is that individuals should speak two or more languages fluently, approximately 50% of the time, ideally daily, at least since their early adulthood. In addition, the findings on the age of the onset of AD are based on questionnaires and interviews with these patients, their family members, or caregivers, which might also contribute to the misinterpretation of these results. Generally, retrospective studies usually have more potential sources of bias and confounding than prospective studies.50

Discussion

As research on bilingualism reveals, there has been an increased interest in how and whether lifelong bilingualism protects the brain from cognitive decline, specifically from the symptoms of AD.51 However, the results in favor of bilingual advantage differ as the findings from the selected research studies shown in Tables 1 and 2. Almost all retrospective studies in Table 2 demonstrate the delay of the onset of dementia by 4.5 years and manifest that bilingualism contributes to the enhancement of cognitive reserve. This has also been confirmed by recent review studies.51,52 Bialystok et al,52 by comparing monolingual and bilingual older individuals on measures of cognitive function and brain structure, emphasize that bilingualism is a protective factor against the symptoms of dementia. Nevertheless, their review leaves out some of the important prospective studies on this topic.36,37,39 The same is true for the study by Alvarez and Rodriguez.53

On the contrary, the findings from the prospective studies exert nonsignificant results as far as the bilingual advantage is concerned. The latest meta-analytic review study provides conclusive results about this.54 Mukadam et al54 on the basis of effect sizes (odds ratio of dementia of 0.96 [95% CI 0.74–1.23] in bilingual participants [n=5,527] compared to monolinguals) from the analyzed studies have concluded that there is no evidence that bilingualism protects from cognitive decline or dementia. The retrospective studies they detected could not be analyzed for effect sizes, since they incline to confounding by education or cultural differences in presentation to dementia services. This seems to be caused by shortcomings in the conception and assessment of bilinguals and confounding variables.55 The most conflicting factors appear to be immigration status, education, group heterogeneity, or social factors.56 In prospective studies, bilingualism is usually connected with a higher level of formal education. However, in retrospective studies, there is no consensus on this issue. In some studies,26,44,45 monolingual groups have higher mean scores on education than bilingual groups. In addition, Fuller-Thomson and Kuh57 point out that immigrants usually have less morbidity and mortality. Therefore, more research should be performed on bilingualism in a nonimmigrant population and their cognitive decline. In addition, all the demographic data about the participants’ background, for example, education, occupation, or immigrant status, should be properly collected.

Thus, the authors of this mini-review agree with Strauss58 that although both types of the studies arrive at conflicting conclusions on bilingualism and its impact on the delay of the onset of dementia, one thing seems to be sure; speaking or learning another language cannot cause any harm. Furthermore, Bak14 adds that it has no side effects, and Bialystok et al52 emphasize its important role in the reduction of potential costs, both for humans and national economy.

The limitations of this review study involve the use of insufficient and different methodologies, especially in the retrospective studies. This might result in the overestimated effects of the findings, which may have an adverse impact on the validity of these reviewed studies.59,60

Conclusion

Although the findings of this mini-review did not arrive at any revolutionary conclusions, they suggest that more research should be conducted on bilingualism and its impact on the delay of the onset of dementia, since the brain studies have brought positive findings as far as the enhancement of cognitive reserve is concerned.

In addition, Kurdziel et al61 say, learning throughout aging is necessary because older individuals who keep mentally and physically active are less likely to be cognitively impaired and depressed.

Acknowledgments

This paper is supported by the project Excellence (2017) at the Faculty of Informatics and Management of the University of Hradec Kralove, Czech Republic, and by the research project PROGRES Q40 run at the Medical Faculty of Charles University.

Disclosure

The authors report no conflicts of interest in this work.


References

1.

United Nations. World population aging [report]. 2015. Available from: http://www.un.org/en/development/desa/population/publications/pdf/ageing/WPA2015_Report.pdf. Accessed May 2, 2017.

2.

Vann MR, Bass PF. The 15 most common health concerns for seniors. 2016. Available from: http://www.everydayhealth.com/news/most-common-health-concerns-seniors/. Accessed May 2, 2017.

3.

Langa KM. Is the risk of Alzheimer’s disease and dementia declining? Alzheimers Res Ther. 2015;7(1):34.

4.

World Health Organization. 10 facts on dementia. 2016. Available from: http://www.who.int/features/factfiles/dementia/en/. Accessed May 2, 2017.

5.

Kirshner HS. Frontotemporal dementia and progressive aphasia, a review. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2014;10:1045–1055.

6.

Klimova B, Maresova P, Valis M, Hort J, Kuca K. Alzheimer’s disease and language impairments: social intervention and medical treatment. Clin Interv Aging. 2015;10:1401–1408.

7.

Maresova P, Klimova B, Kuca K. Alzheimer’s disease: cost cuts call for novel drugs development and national strategy. Ceska Slov Farm. 2015;64:25–30.

8.

Karakaya T, Fußer F, Schröder J, Pantel J. Pharmacological treatment of mild cognitive impairment as a prodromal syndrome of Alzheimer’s disease. Curr Neuropharmacol. 2013;11(1):102–108.

9.

Stern Y. Cognitive reserve: implications for assessment and intervention. Folia Phoniatr Logop. 2013;65(2):49–54.

10.

Antoniou M, Gunasekera GM, Wong PC. Foreign language training as cognitive therapy for age-related cognitive decline: a hypothesis for future research. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2013;37(10 Pt 2):2689–2698.

11.

Fischer A, Sananbenesi F, Wang X, Dobbin M, Tsai LH. Recovery of learning and memory is associated with chromatin remodelling. Nature. 2007;447(7141):178–182.

12.

McClearn GE, Johansson B, Berg S, et al. Substantial genetic influence on cognitive abilities in twins 80 or more years old. Science. 1997;276(5318):1560–1563.

13.

Klimova B, Kuca K. Alzheimer’s disease: potential preventive, non-invasive, intervention strategies in lowering the risk of cognitive decline – a review study. J Appl Biomed. 2015;13(4):257–261.

14.

Bak T. Language lessons to help protect against dementia. BMJ. 2016;354:i5039.

15.

Cheng KW, Deng YH, Li M, Yan HM. The impact of L2 learning on cognitive aging. ADMET DMPK. 2015;3(3):260–273.

16.

Kaiwen C, Yanhui D, Dezhong Y. Bilingualism (multilingualism) helps resist Alzheimer’s disease? Adv Psychol Sci. 2014;22(11):1723–1732.

17.

Bak TH, Nissan JJ, Allerhand MM, Deary IJ. Does bilingualism influence cognitive aging? Ann Neurol. 2014;75(6):959–963.

18.

Goral M, Campanelli L, Spiro A 3rd. Language dominance and inhibition abilities in bilingual older adults. Biling (Camb Engl). 2015;18(1):79–89.

19.

Luk G, Bialystok E, Craik FI, Grady CL. Lifelong bilingualism maintains white matter integrity in older adults. J Neurosci. 2011;31(46):16808–16813.

20.

Schweitzer TA, Ware J, Fischer CE, Craik FI, Bialystok E. Bilingualism as a contributor to cognitive reserve: evidence from brain atrophy in Alzheimer’s disease. Cortex. 2012;48(8):991–996.

21.

Grundy JG, Anderson JAE, Bialystok E. Neural correlates of cognitive processing in monolinguals and bilinguals. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2017;1396(1):183–201.

22.

Abutalebi J, Canini M, Della Rosa PA, Green DW, Weekes BS. The neuroprotective effects of bilingualism upon the inferior parietal lobule: a structural neuroimaging study in aging Chinese bilinguals. J Neurolinguistics. 2015;33:3–13.

23.

Olsen RK, Pangelinan MM, Bogulski C, et al. The effect of lifelong bilingualism on regional grey and white matter volume. Brain Res. 2015;1612:128–139.

24.

Perani D, Farsad M, Ballarini T, et al. The impact of bilingualism on brain reserve and metabolic connectivity in Alzheimer’s disease. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2017;114(7):1690–1695.

25.

Estanga A, Ecay-Torres M, Ibañez A, et al. Beneficial effect of bilingualism on Alzheimer’s disease CSF biomarkers and cognition. Neurobiol Aging. 2017;50:144–151.

26.

Bialystok E, Craik FI, Freedman M. Bilingualism as a protection against the onset of symptoms of dementia. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45(2):459–464.

27.

Lee RR, Tzeng OJ. Neural bilingualism: a new look at an old problem. Lang Linguist. 2016;17(2):147–193.

28.

Schlegel AA, Rudelson JJ, Tse PU. White matter structure changes as adults learn a second language. J Cogn Neurosci. 2012;24(8):1664–1670.

29.

Bialystok E. The structure of age: in search of barriers to second language acquisition. Second Lang Res. 1997;13(2):116–137.

30.

Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG; The PRISMA Group. Preferred reporting items for systematic review and meta-analysis: the PRISMA statement. PLoS Med. 2009;6(6):e1000097.

31.

Alladi S, Bak TH, Shailaja M, et al. Bilingualism delays the onset of behavioural but not aphasic forms of frontotemporal dementia. Neuropsychogia. 2017;99:207–212.

32.

Hindle JV, Martin-Forbes PA, Bastable AJ, et al. Cognitive reserve in Parkinson’s disease: the effects of Welsh-English bilingualism on executive function. Parkinsons Dis. 2015;2015:943572.

33.

Calabria M, Branzi F, Marne P, Hernandez M, Costa A. Age-related effects over bilingual language control and executive control. Biling Lang Cogn. 2015;18(1):65–78.

34.

Chertkow H, Whitehead V, Phillips N, Wolfson C, Atherton J, Bergman H. Multilingualism (but not always bilingualism) delays the onset of Alzheimer disease: evidence from a bilingual community. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2010;24(2):118–125.

35.

Liu YC, Liu YY, Yip PK, Meguro M, Meguro K. Speaking one more language in early life has only minor effects on cognition in Taiwanese with low education level: the Taishan Project. Psychogeriatrics. 2017;17(4):256–261.

36.

Lawton DM, Gasquoine PG, Weimer AA. Age of dementia diagnosis in community dwelling bilingual and monolingual Hispanic Americans. Cortex. 2015;66:141–145.

37.

Sanders AE, Hall CB, Katz MJ, Lipton RB. Non-native language use and risk of incident dementia in the elderly. J Alzheimers Dis. 2012;29(1):99–108.

38.

Wilson RS, Boyle PA, Yang J, James BD, Bennett DA. Early life instruction in foreign language and music and incidence of mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychology. 2015;29(2):292–302.

39.

Yeung CM, St John PD, Menec V, Tyas SL. Is bilingualism associated with a lower risk of dementia in community-living old adults? Cross-sectional and prospective analyses. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2014;28(4):326–332.

40.

Zahodne LB, Schofield PW, Farrell MT, Stern Y, Manly JJ. Bilingualism does not alter cognitive decline or dementia risk among Spanish speaking immigrants. Neuropsychology. 2014;28(2):238–246.

41.

Alladi S, Bak TH, Duggirala V, et al. Bilingualism delays age at onset of dementia, independent of education and immigration status. Neurology. 2013;81(22):1938–1944.

42.

Bialystok E, Craik FI, Binns MA, Ossher L, Freedman M. Effects of bilingualism on the age of onset and progression of MCI and AD: evidence from executive function tests. Neuropsychology. 2014;28(2):290–304.

43.

Clare L, Whitaker CJ, Craik FI, et al. Bilingualism, executive control, and age at diagnosis among people with early-stage Alzheimer’s disease in Wales. J Neuropsychol. 2014;10(2):163–185.

44.

Craik FI, Bialystok E, Freedman M. Delaying the onset of Alzheimer disease: bilingualism as a form of cognitive reserve. Neurology. 2010;75(19):1726–1729.

45.

Gollan TH, Salmon DP, Montoya RI, Galasko DR. Degree of bilingualism predicts age of diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease in low-education but not in highly educated Hispanics. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49(14):3826–3830.

46.

Ossher L, Bialystok E, Craik FI, Murphy KJ, Troyer AK. The effect of bilingualism on amnestic mild cognitive impairment. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2013;68(1):8–12.

47.

Woumans E, Santens P, Sieben A, Versijpt J, Stevens M, Duyck W. Bilingualism delays clinical manifestation of Alzheimer’s disease. Biling Lang Cogn. 2015;18(3):568–574.

48.

Wells GA, Shea B, O’Connell D, et al. The Newcastle-Ottawa Scale (NOS) for assessing the quality of nonrandomized studies in meta-analyses. Available from: http://www.ohri.ca/programs/clinical_epidemiology/oxford.asp. Accessed July 25, 2017.

49.

Bakker M. Power problems: n>138. Cortex. 2015;73:367–368.

50.

Cohort Studies. Prospective versus retrospective cohort studies. 2016. Available from: http://sphweb.bumc.bu.edu/otlt/MPH-Modules/EP/EP713_CohortStudies/EP713_CohortStudies2.html. Accessed May 8, 2017.

51.

Abutalebi J, Clahsen H. Bilingualism, cognition, and aging. Biling Lang Cogn. 2015;18(1):1–2.

52.

Bialystok E, Abutalebi J, Bak TH, Burke DM, Kroll J. Aging in two languages: implications for public health. Ageing Res Rev. 2016;27:56–60.

53.

Alvarez A, Rodriguez M. The effects of bilingualism on cognitive decline in mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and Alzheimer’s disease (AD). Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2016;31(6):594.

54.

Mukadam N, Sommerland A, Livingston G. The relationship of bilingualism compared to monolingualism to the risk of cognitive decline or dementia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Alzheimers Dis. 2017;58(1):45–54.

55.

Paap KR, Johnson HA, Sawi O. Should the search for bilingual advantages in executive functioning continue? Cortex. 2016;74:305–314.

56.

Calvo V, García AM, Manoiloff L, Ibáñez A. Bilingualism and cognitive reserve: a critical overview and a plea for methodological innovations. Front Aging Neurosci. 2016;7:249.

57.

Fuller-Thomson E, Kuh D. The healthy migrant effect may confound the link between bilingualism and delayed onset of Alzheimer’s disease. Cortex. 2014;52:128–130.

58.

Strauss S. Does bilingualism delay dementia? CMAJ. 2015;187(7):E209–E210.

59.

Melby-Lervåg M, Hulme C. There is no convincing evidence that working memory training is effective: a reply to Au et al (2014) and Karbach and Verhaeghen (2014). Psychon Bull Rev. 2016;23(1):324–330.

60.

Melby-Lervåg M, Hulme C. Is working memory training effective? A meta-analytic review. Dev Psychol. 2013;49(2):270–291.

61.

Kurdziel LB, Mantua J, Spencer RM. Novel word learning in older adults: a role for sleep? Brain Lang. 2017;167:106–113.

Creative Commons License © 2017 The Author(s). This work is published and licensed by Dove Medical Press Limited. The full terms of this license are available at https://www.dovepress.com/terms.php and incorporate the Creative Commons Attribution - Non Commercial (unported, v3.0) License. By accessing the work you hereby accept the Terms. Non-commercial uses of the work are permitted without any further permission from Dove Medical Press Limited, provided the work is properly attributed. For permission for commercial use of this work, please see paragraphs 4.2 and 5 of our Terms.